Skip to main content
Original Article

Fading Affect Bias in Intergroup Relations

The Role of Intergroup Contact in Fading Outgroup Affect

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000449

Abstract. Negative affect associated with autobiographical events fades faster over time than positive affect. This Fading Affect Bias (FAB) has been established in the individual and interpersonal domains. Two studies tested the FAB in intergroup relations with Muslims (N= 76 White British non-Muslim) and opposite gender (N = 242 women and men) as target outgroups. The results indicated that the FAB exists in an intergroup context, for both ingroup and outgroup memories. Mediation analyses showed that intergroup contact is related to a lower fading of positive affect associated with the outgroup memory, through greater memory strength and a more positive outgroup member evaluation. The findings are important for understanding affect associated with intergroup memories and the buffering effect of positive contact.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Addison-Wesley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aberson, C. L. (2015). Positive intergroup contact, negative intergroup contact, and threat as predictors of cognitive and affective dimensions of prejudice. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18(6), 743–760. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430214556699 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Árnadóttir, K., Lolliot, S., Brown, R., & Hewstone, M. (2018). Positive and negative intergroup contact: Interaction not asymmetry: Positive and negative contact. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(6), 784–800. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2365 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barlow, F. K., Paolini, S., Pedersen, A., Hornsey, M. J., Radke, H. R., Harwood, J., Rubin, M., & Sibley, C. G. (2012). The contact caveat: Negative contact predicts increased prejudice more than positive contact predicts reduced prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(12), 1629–1643. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212457953 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Birtel, M. D., & Crisp, R. J. (2012). “Treating” prejudice: An exposure-therapy approach to reducing negative reactions toward stigmatized groups. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1379–1386. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443838 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Birtel, M. D., Di Bernardo, G. A., Stathi, S., Crisp, R. J., Cadamuro, A., & Vezzali, L. (2019). Imagined contact reduces prejudice in pre-school children. Social Development, 28(4), 1054–1073. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12374 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429–444. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00126 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, M. B. (2001). Ingroup identification and intergroup conflict: When does ingroup love become outgroup hate? Rutgers series on self and social identity. In R. D. AshmoreL. JussimD. WilderEds., Social identity, intergroup conflict, and conflict reduction (Vol. 3, pp. 17–41). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cason, H. (1932). The learning and retention of pleasant and unpleasant activities. Archives of Psychology, 134, 1–96. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. memory, 16(4), 538–547. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802010463 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, K., Tropp, L. R., Aron, A., Pettigrew, T. F., & Wright, S. C. (2011). Cross-group friendships and intergroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(4), 332–351. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311411103 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18(3), 233–239. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fuochi, G., Voci, A., Boin, J., & Hewstone, M. (2020). Close to me: The importance of closeness vs. superficiality in explaining the positive‐negative contact asymmetry. European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(4), 766–782. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2667 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gibbons, J. A., Lee, S. A., & Walker, W. R. (2011). The fading affect bias begins within 12 hours and persists for 3 months. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 663–672. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1738 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graf, S., Paolini, S., & Rubin, M. (2014). Negative intergroup contact is more influential, but positive intergroup contact is more common: Assessing contact prominence and contact prevalence in five Central European countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(6), 536–547. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2052 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haddock, G., Zanna, M. P., & Esses, V. M. (1993). Assessing the structure of prejudicial attitudes: The case of attitudes toward homosexuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1105–1118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, A. F. (2019). The PROCESS macro for SPSS, SAS, and R. http://processmacro.org/download.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hayward, L. E., Tropp, L. R., Hornsey, M. J., & Barlow, F. K. (2017). Toward a comprehensive understanding of intergroup contact: Descriptions and mediators of positive and negative contact among majority and minority groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(3), 347–364. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216685291 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 575–604. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135109 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holmes, D. S. (1970). Differential change in affective intensity and the forgetting of unpleasant personal experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15(3), 234–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029394 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Home Office. (2019). Hate Crime, England and Wales, 2018/19. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety, perceived out-group variability, and out-group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(6), 700–710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167293196005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindeman, M. I., Zengel, B., & Skowronski, J. J. (2017). An exploration of the relationship among valence, fading affect, rehearsal frequency, and memory vividness for past personal events. memory, 25(6), 724–735. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1210172 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 602–616. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(S1), 3–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meleady, R., & Forder, L. (2019). When contact goes wrong: Negative intergroup contact promotes generalized outgroup avoidance. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(5), 688–707. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430218761568 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meltzer, H. (1930). Individual differences in forgetting pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 21, 399–409. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0073944 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meltzer, H. (1931). Sex differences in forgetting pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 25(6), 450–464. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0071952 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miles, E., & Crisp, R. J. (2014). A meta-analytic test of the imagined contact hypothesis. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 17(1), 3–26. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430213510573 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mummendey, A., & Otten, S. (1998). Positive–negative asymmetry in social discrimination. European Review of Social Psychology, 9(1), 107–143. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779843000063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mummendey, A., & Otten, S. (2001). Aversive discrimination. In R. BrownS. GaertnerEds., Blackwell handbook on social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 112–132). Basil Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Office for National Statistics. (2011). Census: Ethnic group, local authorities in the United Kingdom, 2011. https://www.ons.gov.uk/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 751–783. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta‐analytic tests of three mediators. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 922–934. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.504 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2011). When groups meet: The dynamics of intergroup contact. Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ritchie, T. D., & Batteson, T. J. (2013). Perceived changes in ordinary autobiographical events’ affect and visual imagery colorfulness. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(2), 461–470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.02.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ritchie, T. D., Batteson, T. J., Bohn, A., Crawford, M. T., Ferguson, G. V., Schrauf, R. W., Vogl, R. J., & Walker, W. R. (2015). A pancultural perspective on the fading affect bias in autobiographical memory. Memory, 23(2), 278–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.884138 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ritchie, T. D., Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (2017). Does a person selectively remember the good or the bad from their personal past? It depends on the recall target and the person’s favourability of self-views. Memory, 25(8), 934–944. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1233984 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ritchie, T. D., Skowronski, J. J., Wood, S. E., Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Gibbons, J. A. (2006). Event self-importance, event rehearsal, and the fading affect bias in autobiographical memory. Self and Identity, 5(2), 172–195. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860600591222 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rotella, K. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2013). Motivated to “forget” the effects of in-group wrongdoing on memory and collective guilt. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(6), 730–737. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613482986 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296–320. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sahdra, B., & Ross, M. (2007). Group identification and historical memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(3), 384–395. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206296103 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (2020). In human memory, good can be stronger than bad. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 86–91. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419896363 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J. (2011). The positivity bias and the fading affect bias in autobiographical memory: A Self-motives perspective. In C. SedikidesM. AlickeEds., Handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection (pp. 211–231). Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J., Walker, W. R., Henderson, D. X., & Bond, G. D. (2014). The fading affect bias: Its history, its implications, and its future. In J. M. OlsonM. P. ZannaEds., Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 49, pp. 163–218). Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, 41(3), 157–175. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1985.tb01134.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swart, H., Hewstone, M., Christ, O., & Voci, A. (2011). Affective mediators of intergroup contact: A three-wave longitudinal study in South Africa. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1221–1238. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024450 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149–178. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420010202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflicts. In W. G. AustinS. WorchelEds., The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Brooks-Cole. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M., & Voci, A. (2007). Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: The mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(3), 369–388. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.369 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turner, R. N., Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J., & Cairns, E. (2013). Contact between Catholic and Protestant schoolchildren in Northern Ireland. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43(S2), 216–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voci, A., & Hewstone, M. (2003). Intergroup contact and prejudice toward immigrants in Italy: The mediational role of anxiety and the moderational role of group salience. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6(1), 37–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430203006001011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, C. P. (2003). Life is pleasant – and memory helps to keep it that way!. Review of General Psychology, 7(2), 203–210. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Thompson, C. P. (1997). Autobiographical memory: Unpleasantness fades faster than pleasantness over time. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11(5), 399–413. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199710)11:5<399::AID-ACP462>3.0.CO;2-E First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, W. R., Yanacu, C. N., & Skowronski, J. J. (2014). Trait anxiety reduces affective fading for both positive and negative autobiographical memories. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 10(3), 81–89. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0159-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zengel, B., Lee, E. M., Walker, W. R., & Skowronski, J. J. (2019). Romantic relationships and fading of affect for memories of the shared past. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33(5), 861–872. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3527 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar