Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T20:01:03.282Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessment of emotion word vocabulary and its contribution to reading comprehension

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2023

Tami Sabag-Shushan*
Affiliation:
Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, Department of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel Department of Education, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel Center for the Study of Pedagogy – Research Practice Partnerships, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
Tami Katzir
Affiliation:
Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, Department of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Anat Prior
Affiliation:
Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, Department of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
*
*Corresponding author. Email: Tammisho@gmail.com

Abstract

The contribution of vocabulary to academic achievements in general and to reading comprehension (RC) in particular has led to the development of various tools for vocabulary assessment. However, existing assessments do not distinguish between word types, and specifically, they do not target emotion vocabulary, despite growing recognition of the importance of emotional processing to RC ability. In this study, we first describe the development of a novel vocabulary assessment in Hebrew – Herut – and examine its validity and sensitivity. This assessment includes both emotion words and nonemotion words, and is based on curriculum. Next, we studied the contribution of the emotion and nonemotion words subscales of the Herut to RC in a sample of 1,333 Hebrew-speaking fourth- and fifth-grade students. Both types of vocabulary knowledge made significant independent contributions to RC, and the contribution of emotion words was slightly larger than that of nonemotion words. Finally, the Herut measure was found to be more predictive of RC than a general vocabulary measure in Hebrew.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2010). The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the Affective Simon Task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(3), 310328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(4), 578602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Argaman, O. (2006). The denotations of emotion words and the lexical means for their expression. PhD diss. University of Haifa. [in Hebrew].Google Scholar
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2013). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. Guilford Press, vol. 2 (pp. 19–39).Google Scholar
Beck, L., Kumschick, I. R., Eid, M., & Klann-Delius, G. (2012). Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion, 12(3), 503516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly, 34(2), 213238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coxhead, A. (2011). The Academic Word List ten years on: Research and teaching implications. TESOL Quarterly, 45, 355362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denham, S. A. (2007). Dealing with feelings: How children negotiate the worlds of emotions and social relationships. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 11(1), 148.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M. (1965). Expanded manual for the Peabody picture vocabulary test. American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. American guidance service, Incorporated.Google Scholar
Egoz-Liebstein, T. (2009). Mental verbs in the discourse of children 28 years old. [Unpublished Master’s Thesis]. Tel-Aviv University. [in Hebrew].Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1971, vol. 19 (pp. 207282). University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the human face. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fontaine, J. R., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (Eds.) (2013). Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R., & Plaut, D. (2005). The word-frequency database for printed Hebrew. The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.Google Scholar
Gardner, D., & Davies, M. (2013). A new academic vocabulary list. Applied Linguistics, 35(3), 305327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gendron, M., Lindquist, K. A., Barsalou, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). Emotion words shape emotion percepts. Emotion, 12(2), 314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gough, P. B. & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, M. F., Baumann, J. F., Blachowicz, C. L. Z., Manyak, P., Bates, A., Cieply, C., Davis, J. R., & Von Gunten, H. (2013). Words, words everywhere, but which ones do we teach? Reading Teacher, 67(5), 333346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, W. S. & Nagy, W. E. (1986). Theoretical issues in the investigation of the words of internal report. In Gopnik, I. & Gopnik, M. (Eds.), From models to modules: Studies in cognitive science from the McGill workshops (pp. 2665). Ablex.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1993). Understanding emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 237–246). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hinojosa, J. A., Méndez-Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2010). Looking at emotional words is not the same as reading emotional words: Behavioral and neural correlates. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 748757.Google Scholar
Hobbs, J. R. & Gordon, A. S. (2011). The Deep Lexical Semantics of Emotions. In Khurshid, A. (Ed.), Affective computing and sentiment analysis: Emotion, metaphor and terminology (pp. 2734), Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, C. T., Jacobs, A. M., Citron, F. M., & Conrad, M. (2015). The emotion potential of words and passages in reading Harry Potter – An fMRI study. Brain and Language, 142, 96114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12(1), 1823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E., Schultz, D., & Ackerman, B. P. (1996). Emotional knowledge, social competence and behavior problems in disadvantaged children. Unpublished manuscript. University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Katzir, T. (in press). The feeling of reading in a changing world. In M. Bishop, S. Sorondo & M. Suárez-Orozco (Eds.), Education the global impact, Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Katzir, T., Lipka, O., Prior, A., & Shany, M. (2019). From mapping to intervention, a multi componential model for Teacher Training. Report submitted to the Chief of Science, Israeli Ministry of Education (pp. 1200).Google Scholar
Katzir, T., Schiff, R., & Kim, Y.S (2012). The effects of orthographic consistency on reading development: A within and between cross-linguistic study of fluency and accuracy among fourth grade English- and Hebrew- speaking children. Learning and Individual Differences, 6, 673679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S. (1994). Intelligent testing with the WISC-III. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Peyk, P., & Junghofer, M. (2007). Buzzwords: Early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychological Science, 18(6), 475480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). Emotion and attention in visual word processing – An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 7583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
León, J. A., Dávalos, M. T., Escudero, I., Olmos, R., Morera, Y., & Froufe, M. (2015). Effects of valence and causal direction in the emotion inferences processing during reading: Evidence from a lexical decision task. Annals of Psychology, 31(2), 677686.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. (1993a). The emergence of human emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 223–235). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. (1993b). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 536573). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miall, D. S. (1988). Affect and narrative: A model of response to stories. Poetics, 17(3), 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. L., Gouley, K. K., Seifer, R., Zakriski, A., Eguia, M., & Vergnani, M. (2005). Emotion knowledge skills in low income elementary school children: Associations with social status and peer experiences. Social Development, 14(4), 637651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NAEP: National Assessment of Educational Progress (2009). Reading assessment and item specifications for the 2009 National Assessment of Educational Progress. American Institutes for Research.Google Scholar
NAGB: National Assessment Governing Board (2005). Reading framework for the 2009 National Assessment of Educational Progress. American Institutes for Research.Google Scholar
Neuman, S. B., & Wright, T. S. (2014). The magic of words: Teaching vocabulary in the early childhood classroom. American Educator, 38(2), 413.Google Scholar
Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2008). Emotion and emotion-laden words in the bilingual lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 147164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, P. D., Hiebert, E. H., & Kamil, M. L. (2007). Vocabulary assessment: What we know and what we need to learn. Reading Research Quarterly, 42(2), 282296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, P. D., Palincsar, A. S., Biancarosa, G., & Berman, A. I. (Eds.) (2020). Reaping the rewards of the Reading for Understanding initiative. National Academy of Education.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PPVT-4 Publication Summary Form (2007). Retrieved from: http://images.pearsonclinical.com/images/Products/PPVT-IV/ppvt4.pdf Google Scholar
Primor, L., Pierce, M., & Katzir, T. (2011). Predicting reading comprehension of narrative and expository texts among Hebrew-speaking readers with and without a reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 242268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravid, D. & Egoz-Liebstein, T. (2012). Mental state verbs in Hebrew Lexical. Chelkat Lashon, 43–44, 241–224. [in Hebrew].Google Scholar
Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V., Pimperton, H., & Nation, K. (2011). The role of self-teaching in learning orthographic and semantic aspects of new words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 4770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabag-Shushan, T., & Katzir, T. (2018). Tamar test for assessing reading comprehension. University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Sabag-Shushan, T., Yosefi, G., Katzir, T., & Prior, A. (2018). Herut test for assessing emotion and non-emotion words. University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 44(4), 695729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, N., Nation, P., & Kremmel, B. (2020). Moving the field of vocabulary assessment forward: The need for more rigorous test development and validation. Language Teaching, 53(1), 109120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2012). Emotion words affect eye fixations during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 783792.Google ScholarPubMed
Sedita, J. (2005). Effective vocabulary instruction. Insights on Learning Disabilities, 2(1), 3345.Google Scholar
Seigneuric, A., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Contribution of working memory capacity to children’s reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing, 18(7–9), 617656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shahar-Yames, D., & Prior, A. (2018). The challenge and the opportunity of lexical inferencing in language minority students. Reading and Writing, 31(5), 11091132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shany, M. & Blicher, S. (2017). Literal and homiletic meaning. Reading comprehension test for fourth and fifth grade. Department of Learning Disabilities and Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Shany, M., Prior, A., & Blicher, S. (2017). Vocabulary assessment for elementary school. Department of Learning Disabilities and Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Shatil, E., Nevo, B., & Breznitz, Z. (2007). Elul test. A standardized diagnostic test for learning disabilities. University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Shiota, M. N., & Kelther, D. (2005). What do emotion words represent? Psychological Inquiry, 16(1), 3237.Google Scholar
Silverman, R. D., Proctor, C. P., Harring, J. R., Doyle, B., Mitchell, M. A., & Meyer, A. G. (2014). Teachers’ instruction and students’ vocabulary and comprehension: An exploratory study with English monolingual and Spanish–English bilingual students in Grades 3–5. Reading Research Quarterly, 49(1), 3160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgrsen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). TOWRE: Test of Word Reading Efficiency . Examiner’s manual. PRO-ED.Google Scholar