Skip to main content
Log in

Extraction asymmetries in topic structures: a comparative analysis

  • Published:
Journal of East Asian Linguistics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We review two conflicting accounts of the argument-adjunct (a)symmetry found in topic structures in English and in the Romance languages. Based on the contrast, we examine topic structures in Mandarin Chinese, especially the behaviors of adjunct topics in multiple-topic sentences, embedded clauses, and non-declarative sentences. We observe a systematic extraction asymmetry in these environments between argument and circumstantial-adjunct topics, on the one hand, and manner-adjunct topics, on the other. The new findings from Mandarin Chinese suggest that the argument-adjunct asymmetry of the topic structure in English is only apparent, and the extraction asymmetry should be understood cross-linguistically as a movement-merger asymmetry (Haegeman 2012a: Adverbial clauses, main clause phenomena, and composition of the left periphery, OUP). In this respect, we propose that argument topics and circumstantial-adjunct topics in Chinese are base-generated in the fronted topic position, and manner-adjunct topics are results of syntactic movements. The topicalization asymmetry in Mandarin Chinese can be further unified with Tsai’s (1994: On economizing the theory of A-bar dependencies, MIT dissertation) theory of the nominal-adverbial asymmetry of wh-dependencies in Chinese, where argument wh-phrases and circumstantial wh-phrases (including time, location, and instrumental wh-phrases) also resort to the base-generation strategy (through merger and LF binding), and manner-adverbial wh-phrases make use of the movement strategy (through LF movement).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. ‘Circumstantial adjunct’ (c-adjunct) is a descriptive term for modifiers that are used to describe the time, location, instrument, or comitative participant of the event (Cinque 1999). While it may include manner adjuncts in descriptive grammars, we argue that, morphosyntactically, manner adverbs do not behave the same way as other types of c-adjuncts, and should not be categorized as a type of c-adjunct (at least in Chinese). In Chinese, such ‘m-adjuncts’ are morphologically distinct from other c-adjuncts. Manner adverbs/m-adverbs are always introduced by the adverbial marker –de, while other types of c-adjuncts are either bare or introduced by a preposition. Syntactically, m-adverbs are closely attached to the verb (unless the m-adverb is topicalized), and m-adverbs cannot be separated from the VP by a negation or any other adverb, while c-adjuncts are not subject to the same conditions. Lastly, m-adverbs block the A-not-A form on the verb, but c-adjuncts, such as temporal or locational adjuncts, do not (Law 2006).

  2. Unless otherwise specified, the grammatical judgments of Mandarin Chinese in this paper are based on the authors’ own native intuitions, and we have confirmed our judgments with four other native speakers.

  3. The adverbial suffix –de (地 in Chinese character) is in complementary distribution with the adjectival suffix –de (的 in Chinese character).

  4. Adopting Starke (2001), Boeckx and Jeong (2004), Rizzi (2004), Endo (2007) and Abels (2012), Haegeman (2012a) proposes that intervention is caused by feature identity (matching features) or feature inclusion (the intervenor carries features in addition to the matching ones). A topic is a stronger intervenor than the world operator movement because it carries a discourse feature [δ] in addition to the shared operator [Op] feature (which triggers the movement).

  5. As pointed out by an anonymous reviewer, one aspect where the m-adjunct topic differs from other types of topics is in their clause-boundedness:

    (i)

    {??*Toutoumomo-de},

    [wo

    kanjian

    [Lisi

    {toutoumomo-de}

    pao-hui

    jia]].

      furtively

    1sg

    see

    Lisi

    furtively

    run-back

    home

       ‘I saw that Lisi ran back home furtively.’

    (i) is marginal or even unacceptable for most native speakers we consulted. The reviewer points out that the non-cyclic nature of m-adjunct fronting may argue against a movement analysis. While we do not know what factor leads to the clause-boundedness of adverbial fronting, it is important to note that the clause-boundedness of adverb fronting is not restricted to Mandarin Chinese. For example, Simple Preposing (SP) (e.g., AdvP/PP-topicalization without a clitic) in Italian is also clause-bounded (Rizzi 2004: 249):

    (ii)

    Rapidamente, (*Gianni dice che) hanno risolto il problema.

    ‘Rapidly, (Gianni says that) they solved the problem.’

    Furthermore, like m-adjunct topics in Chinese, SP is also a main clause phenomenon and does not allow iteration (Cruschina 2010: 64–66; Rizzi and Bocci 2017: 6). Since SP is subject to intervention and does not employ clitic resumption, these facts strongly suggest that SP is also derived from movement. We tentatively follow Rizzi (2004) in assuming that all of these movements target ModP and they interact with various topic phrases through further movements or agreement.

  6. An anonymous reviewer points out that an m-adjunct cannot occur with the A-not-A form on the verb without any displacement to sentence-initial position, as in (i), which might suggest that the movement of the m-adjunct is irrelevant to our question:

    (i)

    *Ta

    manman-de

    pao-bu-pao?

    3sg

    slowly

    run-neg-run

    While the ungrammaticality of (i) remains an interesting problem (but see Law 2006 and Simpson 2015 for a focus intervention account), we contend that (i) is different from (44)/(46). The A-not-A operator in (44)/(46) is attached to the modal auxiliary, which can form a legitimate A-not-A question when the m-adverb does not move, as shown in (44). Therefore, the ungrammaticality of (46) must have been caused by the movement (and intervention) of the m-adverb.

  7. Badan and Del Gobbo (2011: 74) claim that in a multiple-topic construction, topics cannot be resumed by resumptive pronouns in Chinese:

    (i)

    *Zhe

    jian

    yinhang,

    Zhangsan,

    wo

    zhidao

    women

    keyi

    this

    clf

    bank

    Zhagnsan

    1sg

    know

    1pl

    can

    cong

    nali

    ti

    ta

    jiedao

    hen

    duo

    qian.

    from

    there

    for

    3sg

    borrow

    very

    much

    money

    (intended:) ‘From this bank, for Zhangsan, I know we can borrow a lot of money.’

    However, the following sentence sounds much better, suggesting that (i) might be infelicitous due to some pragmatic reason:

    (ii)

    Zhe

    jian

    yinhang,

    xiang

    Zhangsan

    zheme

     

    this

    clf

    bank

    like

    Zhangsan

    so

     

    qiong

    de

    ren,

    women

    hen

    nan

     

    poor

    DE

    person

    1pl

    very

    hard

     

    ti

    ta

    cong

    nali

    jiedao

    ban-mao

    qian.

    for

    3sg

    from

    there

    borrow

    any

    money

     

    ‘As for this bank, a poor man like Zhangsan, we can hardly borrow any money for him from

     

    there.’

  8. Two anonymous reviewers point out that a sentence like (55b) is acceptable if we use jiu-zhe-yang ‘just-this-way’ or zhe-yang-de ‘this.way-DE’:

    (i)

    Yiwuyishi-de,

    Lisi

    jiu-zhe-yang/?zhe-yang-de

    shuo-chu

    shihua.

    thoroughly

    Lisi

    just-this-way/this.way-DE

    say-out

    truth

    ‘(Just) like this, Lisi told the truth completely.’

    However, we contend that jiu-zhe-yang ‘just-this-way’ or zhe-yang-de ‘this-way-DE’ in (i) is not used as a resumptive pronoun, but it has a referential meaning closer to ‘(just) like this/that’ in English, which refers to some aforementioned manner. Evidence for this claim comes from the fact that the manner adverb can co-occur with these expressions when it is not moved, as in (ii) (which may have a referential cataphoric meaning, as pointed out by a reviewer). This is different from the typical resumptive pronouns in (iii), which cannot be used if the target NP is not topicalized:

    (ii)

    Lisi

    jiu-zhe-yang/?zhe-yang-de

    yiwuyishi-de

    shuo-chu

    shihua.

    Lisi

    just-this-way/this-way-DE

    thoroughly

    say-out

    truth

    ‘(Just) like this, Lisi told the truth completely.’

    (iii)

    a.

    {Zhangsan},

    Lisi

    hen

    taoyan

    tai

    {*Zhangsani}.

       

    Zhangsan

    Lisi

    very

    dislike

    3sg

    Zhangsan

       

    ‘Zhangsan, Lisi does not like him very much.’

    b.

    {Qu-nian

    xiatiani},

    wo

    dangshii

    {*qu-nian

    xiatiani}

    hai

    bu-renshi

    Lisi.

    last-year

    summer

    1sg

    then

    last-year

    summer

    still

    neg-know

    Lisi

    ‘I did not know Lisi yet last summer.’

Abbreviations

SFP:

Sentence-final particle

EXP:

Experiential aspect

References

  • Abels, Klaus. 2012. The Italian left periphery: A view from locality. Linguistic Inquiry 43(2): 229–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexopoulou, Theodora, Edit Doron, and Caroline Heycock. 2004. Broad subject and clitic left dislocation. In Peripheries: Syntactic edges and their effects, ed. David Adger, Cecile De Cat, and George Tsoulas, 329–358. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Aoun, Joseph, and Yen-hui Audrey. Li. 1993. Wh-elements in situ: Syntax or LF? Linguistic Inquiry 24(2): 199–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badan, Linda, and Francesca Del Gobbo. 2011. On the syntax of topic and focus in Chinese. In Mapping the left periphery, ed. Paola Benincà and Nicola Munaro, 63–90. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bhatt, Rajesh, and Roumyana Pancheva. 2006. Conditionals. In The Blackwell companion to syntax, ed. Martin Everaert and Henk van Riemsdijk, 638–687. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi, Valentina, and Mara Frascarelli. 2010. Is topic a root phenomenon? Iberia An Interntional Journal of Theoretical Linguistics 2: 43–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, Cedric, and Youngmi Jeong. 2004. “The fine structure of intervention in syntax.” In Issues in current linguistic theory: A festschrift for Hong Bae Lee, edited by C. Kwon and W. Lee, 83–116. Seoul: Kyungjin.

  • Bruening, Benjamin. 2010. Ditransitive asymmetries and a theory of idiom formation. Linguistic Inquiry 41(4): 519–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chou, Chao-Ting Tim. 2013. Unvalued interpretable features and topic A-movement in Chinese raising modal constructions. Lingua 123: 118–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1999. Adverbs and functional heads: A cross-linguistic perspective. Oxford studies in comparative syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Collins, Chris. 1991. “Why and how come.” MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 15: 31–45.

  • Cruschina, Silvio. 2010. Syntactic extraposition and clitic resumption in Italian. Lingua 120(1): 50–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Culicover, Peter. 1991. Topicalization, inversion, and complementizers in English. In Going Romance and beyond, ed. Denis Delfitto, Martin Everaert, Arnold Evers, and Frits Stuurman, 1–45. Utrecht: University of Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demirdache, Hamida, and Myriam Uribe-Etxebarria. 2004. The syntax of time adverbs. In The syntax of time, ed. Jacqueline Gueron and Jacqueline Lecarme, 143–180. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emonds, Joseph. 1976. A transformation approach to English syntax. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endo, Yoshio. 2007. Locality and information structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Engels, Eva. 2012. Optimizing adverb positions. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, Thomas. 1994. Conditions on Chinese A-not-A questions. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 3(3): 241–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, Thomas. 2002. The syntax of adjuncts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frascarelli, Mara, and Roland Hinterhölzl. 2007. Types of topics in German and Italian. In On information structure, meaning and form, ed. Kerstin Schwabe and Susanne Winkler, 87–116. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, Werner. 2003. Syntactic conditions on adjunct class. In Modifying adjuncts, ed. Ewald Lang, et al., 163–209. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2004. Topicalization, CLLD and the left Periphery. ZAS Papers in Linguistics 35: 157–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2006. Conditionals, factives and the left periphery. Lingua 116(10): 1651–1669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2010. The movement derivation of conditional clauses. Linguistic Inquiry 41(4): 595–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2012a. Adverbial clauses, main clause phenomena, and composition of the left periphery. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2012b. The syntax of MCP: Deriving the truncation account. In Main clause phenomena: New horizons, ed. Lobke Aelbrecht, Liliane Haegeman, and Rachel Nye, 113–134. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, Liliane. 2014. “Locality and the distribution of main clause phenomena.” In Locality, edited by Enoch Olade Aboh, Maria Teresa Guasti, and Ian Roberts, 186–222. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Han, Chung-Hye. 1998. “The structure and interpretation of imperatives: mood and force in Universal Grammar.” Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

  • Hooper, Joan B., and Sandra A. Thompson. 1973. On the applicability of root transformations. Linguistic Inquiry 4(4): 465–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, C.-T. James. 1984. On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 15(4): 531–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, C.-T. James. 1991. Modularity and Chinese A-not-A questions. In Interdisciplinary approaches to languages, ed. Carol Georgopoulos and Roberta Ishihara, 305–322. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, C.-T. James., Yen-hui Audrey. Li, and Yafei Li. 2009. The syntax of Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, C.-T. James, and Chen-sheng Liu. 2000. “Logophoricity, Attitudes, and Ziji at the Interface.” In Long-Distance Reflexive, edited by Peter Cole, Gabriella Hermon, and C. T. James Huang, 141–195. New York: Academic Press.

  • Jackendoff, Ray. 1972. Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kao, Grant Hung-Ta. 2013. “Subjunctive Mood and Main Clause Phenomena in Mandarin Chinese.” MA Thesis, National Tsing Hua University.

  • Kaufmann, Magdalena. 2012. Interpreting imperatives. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krifka, Manfred. 2008. Basic notions of information structure. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 55(3–4): 243–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambrecht, Knud. 1994. Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representations of discourse referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Larson, Richard. 1990. Extraction and multiple selection in PP. The Linguistic Review 7(2): 169–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law, Paul. 2006. Adverbs in A-not-A questions in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 15(2): 97–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, Charles, and Sandra Thompson. 1981. Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, Yen-hui Audrey. 2003. Making sense of language differences. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 22: 23–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maienborn, Claudia. 2001. On the position and interpretation of locative modifiers. Natural Language Semantics 9(2): 191–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, James. 2006. Resumption. In The Blackwell companion to syntax, ed. Martin Everaert and Henk van Riemsdijk, 94–117. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Miyagawa, Shigeru. 2017. Topicalization. Gengo Kenkyu 152: 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pan, Haihua. 1998. Closeness, prominence, and binding theory. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 16(4): 771–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, Victor Junnan. 2016. Resumptivity in Mandarin Chinese: A minimalist account. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, Tanya. 1981. Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. Philosophica 27(1): 53–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1990. Relativized minimality. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1997. The fine structure of the left periphery. In Elements of grammar, ed. Liliane Haegeman, 281–337. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 2004. Locality and left periphery. In Structures and beyond, ed. Adriana Belletti, 223–251. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi, and Giuliano Bocci. 2017. “Left periphery of the clause: Primarily illustrated for Italian.” In The Wiley Blackwell companion to syntax, edited by Martin Everaert and Henk van Riemsdijk. Oxford: Wiley.

  • Shi, Dingxu. 2000. Topic and topic-comment constructions in Mandarin Chinese. Language 76(2): 383–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shyu, Shu-ing. 2014. “Topic and Focus.” In The Handbook of Chinese Linguistics, edited by C. -T. James Huang, Yen-hui Audrey Li, and Andrew Simpson, 100–125. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Simpson, Andrew. 2015. “Verbal answer to yes/no questions, focus, and ellipsis.” In Chinese syntax in a cross-linguistic perspective, edited by Audrey Li, Andrew Simpson, and Wei-tien Dylan Tsai, 300–333. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Starke, Michal. 2001. “Move dissolves into merge: A theory of locality.” Ph.D. dissertation, University of Geneva.

  • Tomaszewicz, Barbara. 2012. The syntactic position of Polish by and Main Clause Phenomena. In Main clause phenomena: New horizons, ed. Lobke Aelbrecht, Liliane Haegeman, and Rachel Nye, 257–278. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, Wei-Tien Dylan. 1994. “On economizing the theory of A-bar dependencies.” Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge.

  • Tsai, Wei-Tien Dylan. 1999. On lexical courtesy. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 8(1): 39–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, Wei-Tien Dylan. 2008. Left periphery and how-why alternations. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 17(2): 83–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Hsiao-Hung Iris. 2008. “Generalized Inversion and the Theory of Agree.” Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge.

  • Xu, Liejiong. 2004. Manifestation of informational focus. Lingua 114(3): 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Liejiong. 2006. Topicalization in Asian languages. In The Blackwell companion to syntax, ed. Martin Everaert and Henk van Riemsdijk, 137–174. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Liejiong, and Donald T. Langendoen. 1985. Topic structures in Chinese. Language 61(1): 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Barry Chung-Yu. 2012. Intervention effects and wh-construals. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 21(1): 43–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

This research is supported by the research grant of National Science and Technology Council in Taiwan (then Ministry of Science and Technology) [On the Representation and Derivation of Topicalization: #104-2628-H-001-003-]. Earlier versions of the paper have been presented in the ILAS Faculty Lecture (Academia Sinica), the Workshop on Formal Syntax and Semantics (National Tsing Hua University), the 19th SICOGG (Seoul National University), IACL-25 (Eötvös Loránd University), the 53rd Linguistics Colloquium (Nanzan University), and the 5th NAFOSTED Conference on Information and Computer Science (Ton Duc Thang University). We would like to thank the audience and our colleagues at those venues for their comments and questions. We have also benefited greatly from discussion and correspondence with Nigel Duffield, Miao-ling Hsieh, Sam Jheng, Man-ki Lee, Jonah Lin, Mamoru Saito, Hubert Truckenbrodt, Dylan Tsai, and Iris Wang. The authors would like to thank as well two anonymous reviewers and the Editors of Journal of East Asian Linguistics for their helpful comments and remarks. Our special thanks go to Andrew Simpson, whose detailed suggestions have led to many substantial improvements in our presentation and analysis in the paper. Of course, all errors remain the responsibility of the authors alone.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wei-wen Roger Liao.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Liao, Ww.R., Kao, G.HT. Extraction asymmetries in topic structures: a comparative analysis. J East Asian Linguist 32, 63–90 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-023-09252-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-023-09252-y

Keywords

Navigation