Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T14:36:11.170Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reading in kindergarten Arabic-speaking children with low linguistic skills: A longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 April 2023

Jasmeen Mansour-Adwan*
Affiliation:
The Unit for the study of Arabic language, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Study of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel Department of Learning Disabilities, Faculty of Education, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Yasmin Shalhoub-Awwad
Affiliation:
The Unit for the study of Arabic language, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Study of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel Department of Learning Disabilities, Faculty of Education, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Ravit Cohen-Mimran
Affiliation:
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Asaid Khateb*
Affiliation:
The Unit for the study of Arabic language, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center for the Study of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel Department of Learning Disabilities, Faculty of Education, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
*
*Corresponding authors. Emails: jasmeenmansour@gmail.com; akhateb@edu.haifa.ac.il
*Corresponding authors. Emails: jasmeenmansour@gmail.com; akhateb@edu.haifa.ac.il

Abstract

The present longitudinal study aimed to explore the connections between different linguistic profiles at kindergarten and reading achievements at first grade. These profiles are based on the two-dimensional model (Bishop & Snowling, 2004), which associates reading skills with phonological and other language abilities. This model was examined mainly in Indo-European languages but scarcely in Arabic. Arabic-speaking children were assigned to four linguistic profiles in kindergartens: low language (LL; N = 111), low phonology (LPh; N = 120), low language and low phonology (LLLPh; N = 139), and typical language and typical phonology (TLTPh; N = 135). Multivariate analysis was used to compare their reading achievements at first grade, and the overlap between linguistic and reading profiles was estimated. The results revealed significant differences between the different linguistic profiles in all reading measures. LLLPh group gained lower scores in reading tasks compared to the other groups. Significant relationships have been found between linguistic and reading profiles indicating reading difficulties among 14.5% of the children from TLTPh, 63% of LLLPh, 35% of LL, and 35.6% of LPh. The findings support the relationship between low linguistic skills and reading difficulties and emphasize the potential roles of both phonological and language skills for reading.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Ahmad, H., Ibrahim, R., & Share, D. (2014) Cognitive Predictors of Early Reading Ability in Arabic: A Longitudinal Study from Kindergarten to Grade 2. In Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Joshi, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy: Literacy Studies (Perspectives from Cognitive Neurosciences, Linguistics, Psychology and Education) (pp. 171194). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abu-Ahmad, H., Jabbour-Danial, M., Mansour-Adwan, J., Joubran-Awwadia, N., Mutlak-dahoud, R., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2019). Reading CV units in Arabic [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Abu-Ahmad, H., Jabbour-Danial, M., Mansour-Adwan, J., Joubran-Awwadia, N., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018a). Phonological awareness tasks for Arabic: Final sound isolation [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Abu-Ahmad, H., Jabbour-Danial, M., Mansour-Adwan, J., Joubran-Awwadia, N., Yasin, R., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018b). Phonological awareness tasks for Arabic: First sound isolation [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Abu-Rabia, S. (1996). The role of vowels and context in the reading of highly skilled native Arabic readers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(6), 629641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abu-Rabia, S. (2007). The role of morphology and short vowelization in reading Arabic among normal and dyslexic readers in grades 3, 6, 9, and 12. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(2), 89106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. S. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 16(5), 423442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adlof, S. M., & Hogan, T. P. (2018). Understanding dyslexia in the context of developmental language disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(4), 762773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asaad, H., & Eviatar, Z. (2014). Learning to read in Arabic: The long and winding road. Reading and Writing, 27(4), 649664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asadi, I. A., & Khateb, A. (2017). Predicting reading in vowelized and unvowelized Arabic script: An investigation of reading in first and second grades. Reading Psychology, 38(5), 486505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asadi, I. A., Khateb, A., Ibrahim, R., & Taha, H. (2017). How do different cognitive and linguistic variables contribute to reading in Arabic? A cross-sectional study from first to sixth grade. Reading and Writing, 40, 18351867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asadi, I. A., Khateb, A., & Shany, M. (2016). How simple is reading in Arabic? A cross-sectional investigation of reading comprehension from first to sixth grade. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(S1), S1S22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asadi, I. A., & Shany, M. (2018). Examining the double-deficit hypothesis in vowelized–transparent A rabic in a national representative sample of G rades 3 and 4. Dyslexia, 24(3), 234249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-On, A., Shalhoub-Awwad, Y., & Ibraheem, R. T.-B. (2018). Contribution of phonological and morphological information in reading Arabic: A developmental perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(6), 12531277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batnini, S., & Uno, A. (2015). Investigation of basic cognitive predictors of reading and spelling abilities in Tunisian third-grade primary school children. Brain and Development, 37(6), 579591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, Dorothy V. M, McDonald, D., Bird, S., & Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. (2009). Children who read words accurately despite language impairment: Who are they and how do they do it? Child Development, 80(2), 593605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, Dorothy V. M. (1991). 1. Developmental reading disabilities: The role of phonological processing has been overemphasized. Mind and Language, 6(2), 97101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, Dorothy V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(7), 10271050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, Dorothy V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudelaa, S., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2020). Matrices of the frequency and similarity of Arabic letters and allographs. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 18931905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 13781396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(4), 331361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, P. T. (1992). The syllabic origin of writing and the segmental origin of the alphabet. The Linguistics of Literacy, 21, 83–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, P. T. (2018). An exploration of writing. Equinox Publishing Limited.Google Scholar
De Groot, B. J. A., den Bos, K. P., der Meulen, B. F., & Minnaert, A. E. M. G. (2015). Rapid naming and phonemic awareness in children with reading disabilities and/or specific language impairment: differentiating processes? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(5), 15381548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16(4), 186202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eberhard, D. M., Simons, G. F., & Fennig, C. D. (2019). Ethnologue: languages of the world. Dallas, Texas: SIL International. Online Version: Http://Www.Ethnologue.Com.Google Scholar
El Akiki, C., & Content, A. (2020). Early Sensitivity to Morphology in Beginning Readers of Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(552315), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). Why is it hard to read Arabic? In E. Saiegh-Haddad & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy: Literacy Studies (Perspectives from Cognitive Neurosciences, Linguistics, Psychology and Education) (pp. 7796). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 15(2), 325340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Liberman, I. Y., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Fowler, A. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 263329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U. (2010). A psycholinguistic grain size view of reading development across languages. In Brunswick, N., McDougall, S., and de Mornay Davies, P. (Eds.), Reading and Dyslexia in Different Orthographie (pp. 2342). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Grosvald, M., Al-Alami, S., & Idrissi, A. (2019). Word reading in Arabic: Influences of diacritics and ambiguity. In Stockwell, R., O’Leary, M., XU, Z., & Zhou, Z. L. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 176181). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. www.lingref.com , document#3460. Google Scholar
Hansen, G. F. (2014). Word recognition in Arabic: approaching a language-specific reading model. In Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Joshi, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy. Literacy Studies (Perspectives from Cognitive Neurosciences, Linguistics, Psychology and Education) (pp. 5576). Springer.Google Scholar
Hende, N. (2012). Predicting second grade reading from kindergarten language and literacy in Arabic (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Tel-Aviv University.Google Scholar
Hermena, E. W., Drieghe, D., Hellmuth, S., & Liversedge, S. P. (2015). Processing of Arabic diacritical marks: Phonological-Syntactic disambiguation of homographic verbs and visual crowding effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(2), 494507.Google ScholarPubMed
Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of Arabic orthography slow its processing. Neuropsychology, 16(3), 322–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jabbour-Danial, M., Abu-Ahmad, H., Mansour-Adwan, J., Joubran-Awwadia, N., Yassin, R, & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018). Phonological task of Arabic CV isolation [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Jabbour-Danial, M., Mansour-Adwan, J., Abu-Ahmad, H., Joubran-Awwadia, N., Mutlak-Dahoud, R., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2019). Word reading in Arabic – restricted (maꭍku:l) [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Joubran-Awwadia, N., Mansour-Adwan, J., Jabbour-Danial, M., Abu-Ahmad, H., Mutlak-Dahoud, R., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2019). Pseudoword reading in Arabic [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Joubran-Awwadia, N., Mansour-Adwan, J., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018a). Noun Pluralization test for Arabic [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Joubran-Awwadia, N., Mansour-Adwan, J., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018b). Verb Inflection test for Arabic using Pseudo Root (ꭍ.l.z) [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A. G., & Catts, H. W. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51(4), 337347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khateb, A., Khateb-Abdelgani, M., Taha, H. Y., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). The impact of orthographic connectivity on visual word recognition in Arabic: A cross-sectional study. Reading and Writing, 27(8), 14131436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khateb, A., Taha, H. Y., Elias, I., & Ibrahim, R. (2013). The effect of the internal orthographic connectivity of written Arabic words on the process of the visual recognition: A comparison between skilled and dyslexic readers. Writing Systems Research, 5(2), 214233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2017). Study on morphological awareness and rapid automatized naming through word reading and comprehension in normal and disabled reading Arabic-speaking children. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 33(2), 123140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahfoudhi, A., Elbeheri, G., Al-Rashidi, M., & Everatt, J. (2010). The role of morphological awareness in reading comprehension among typical and learning disabled native Arabic speakers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(6), 500514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mannai, H. A., & Everatt, J. (2005). Phonological processing skills as predictors of literacy amongst Arabic speaking Bahraini children. Dyslexia, 11(4), 269291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansour-Adwan, J., Asadi, I. A., & Khateb, A. (2020). Phonological task comparability in Arabic and relation to reading: a longitudinal assessment in kindergarten and first grade. Reading and Writing, 33(8), 21212151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mansour-Adwan, J., Cohen-Mimran, R., & shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018b). Sentence Repetition test for Arabic [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Mansour-Adwan, J., Cohen-Mimran, R., & shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018c). Receptive Syntax Test for Arabic [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Mansour-Adwan, J., Jabbour-Danial, M., Joubran-Awwadia, N., Abu-Ahmad, H., Mutlak-Dahoud, R., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2019). Word reading in Arabic – unrestricted (muꭍkal) [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Mansour-Adwan, J., Joubran-Awwadia, N., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018a). Verb Derivation test for Arabic [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Mansour-Adwan, J., Joubran-Awwadia, N., & Shalhoub-Awwad, Y. (2018d). Derivation of resultative adjectives from verbs-Arabic test [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, V. T., Heath, S. M., & Mengler, E. D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(7), 869874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, K. K. (2020). How we fail children with developmental language disorder. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(4), 981992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKague, M., Pratt, C., & Johnston, M. B. (2001). The effect of oral vocabulary on reading visually novel words: A comparison of the dual-route-cascaded and triangle frameworks. Cognition, 80(3), 231262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokhtari, K., & Thompson, H. B. (2006). How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth graders. Literacy Research and Instruction, 46(1), 7394.Google Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 134147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, H. M., Hulme, C., Gooch, D., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Preschool language profiles of children at family risk of dyslexia: continuities with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(9), 958968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S. H., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 10311039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72(3), 816833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 212218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., & Van Der Lely, H. K. J. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: towards a multidimensional model. Brain, 136(2), 630645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford Psychologists Press,  The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(3), 431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2013). A tale of one letter: Morphological processing in early Arabic spelling. Writing Systems Research, 5(2), 169188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2018). MAWRID: A model of Arabic word reading in development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(5), 454462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21(5), 481504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Henkin-Roitfarb, R. (2014). The structure of Arabic language and orthography. In Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Joshi, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy. Literacy Studies (Perspectives from Cognitive Neurosciences, Linguistics, Psychology and Education) (pp. 328). Springer.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Spolsky, B. (2014). Acquiring literacy in a diglossic context: Problems and prospects. In Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Joshi, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Arabic Literacy. Literacy Studies (Perspectives from Cognitive Neurosciences, Linguistics, Psychology and Education) (pp. 225240). Springer.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Taha, H. (2017). The role of morphological and phonological awareness in the early development of word spelling and reading in typically developing and disabled Arabic readers. Dyslexia, 23(4), 345371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61(6), 17281743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiff, R., & Lotem, E. (2011). Effects of phonological and morphological awareness on children’s word reading development from two socioeconomic backgrounds. First Language, 31(2), 139163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2007). Morphological analogies in Hebrew-speaking university students with dyslexia compared with typically developing gradeschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(3), 237253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiff, R., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2018). Development and relationships between phonological awareness, morphological awareness and word reading in spoken and standard Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2000). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (UK [CELF-3UK]). London: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Shalev-Laifer, C., Reznik-Nevet, L., & Share, D. (2016). SHALAZ-morphological test for Hebrew [Unpublished test]. Department of Learning Disabilities, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Shalhoub-Awwad, Y., & Leikin, M. (2016). The lexical status of the root in processing morphologically complex words in Arabic. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(4), 296310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shany, M., & Breznitz, Z. (2011). Rate-and accuracy-disabled subtype profiles among adults with dyslexia in the Hebrew orthography. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(7), 889913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shany, M., & Share, D. L. (2011). Subtypes of reading disability in a shallow orthography: a double dissociation between accuracy-disabled and rate-disabled readers of Hebrew. Annals of Dyslexia, 61(1), 6484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: the perils of overreliance on an” outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L., & Blum, P. (2005). Syllable splitting in literate and preliterate Hebrew speakers: Onsets and rimes or bodies and codas? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(2), 182202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. (1998). Dyslexia as a phonological deficit: Evidence and implications. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 3(1), 411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2000). Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 587600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J. (2008). Specific disorders and broader phenotypes: The case of dyslexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 142156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74(2), 358373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Nash, H. M., & Hulme, C. (2020). Dyslexia and Developmental Language Disorder: comorbid disorders with distinct effects on reading comprehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(6), 672680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Nash, H. M., Gooch, D. C., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Hulme, C., Language, W., & Team, R. P. (2019). Developmental outcomes for children at high risk of dyslexia and children with developmental language disorder. Child Development, 90(5), e548--e564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 2453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tadmor-Troyansky, M. (2019). Phonological Awareness in the Hebrew abjad: Consonants Versus Vowels [Doctoral dissertation, University of Haifa]. University of Haifa Digital Archive. https://haifa.userservices.exlibrisgroup.com/discovery/delivery/972HAI_MAIN:AlmaGeneralView/12239575860002791?lang=he&viewerServiceCode=AlmaViewer Google Scholar
Taha, H., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2016). The role of phonological versus morphological skills in the development of Arabic spelling: An intervention study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45(3), 507535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taha, H., & Taha, H. (2019). Morpho-orthographic preferences among typical and poor native Arab readers. Writing Systems Research, 11(2), 212225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taibah, N. J., & Haynes, C. W. (2011). Contributions of phonological processing skills to reading skills in Arabic speaking children. Reading and Writing, 24(9), 10191042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talli, I., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Stavrakaki, S. (2016). Specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: What are the boundaries? Data from Greek children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 49, 339353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tannock, R. (2013). Rethinking ADHD and LD in DSM-5: Proposed changes in diagnostic criteria. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavor, A. (2008). Tavor vocabulary test. In Tel Aviv, Israel: Rayten. Tel Aviv, Israel: Rayten.Google Scholar
Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R. (2017). Morphological awareness: Construct and predictive validity in Arabic. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(5), 10191043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R. (2018). Investigating phonological awareness and naming speed as predictors of reading in Arabic. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 7084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R. (2019). Reading in Arabic: How well does the standard model apply? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(4), 9931014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toppelberg, C. O., & Shapiro, T. (2000). Language disorders: A 10-year research update review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(2), 143152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torppa, M., Eklund, K., van Bergen, E., & Lyytinen, H. (2015). Late-emerging and resolving dyslexia: A follow-up study from age 3 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(7), 13891401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 308321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wattad, H., & Abu Rabia, S. (2020). The Advantage of Morphological Awareness Among Normal and Dyslexic Native Arabic Readers: A Literature Review. Reading Psychology, 41(3), 130156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Line, E. A., Watt, H. J., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Qualitative aspects of developmental language impairment relate to language and literacy outcome in adulthood. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 44(4), 489510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. R., Beitchman, J. H., Johnson, C., Douglas, L., Atkinson, L., Escobar, M., & Wilson, B. (2002). Young adult academic outcomes in a longitudinal sample of early identified language impaired and control children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 635645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21(4), 551559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Mansour-Adwan et al. supplementary material

Appendices

Download Mansour-Adwan et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 244.1 KB