Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T10:32:43.891Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of L1 orthographic depth and L2 proficiency on mapping orthography to phonology in L2-English: an ERP investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 April 2023

Mona Roxana Botezatu*
Affiliation:
Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Abstract

English monolinguals (Experiment 1) and first language (L1)-dominant, Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals (Experiment 2), who differed in L1 orthographic depth (shallow: Spanish; deep: Chinese) and second language (L2–English) proficiency, decided whether visually presented letter strings were English words, while behavioral and EEG measures were recorded. The spelling-sound regularity and consistency of stimuli were covaried such that words had either regular/consistent (e.g., GATE) or irregular/inconsistent mappings (e.g., PINT). Irregular/inconsistent words elicited more positive P200 and less negative N400 amplitudes than regular/consistent words in monolinguals, yet only a P200 response in bilinguals. English proficiency modulated L2 reading strategies, such that bilinguals employed distinct reading unit sizes in the L2 than the L1 when L2 proficiency was low, but transferred L1 reading units to the L2 when L2 proficiency was high. ERP results suggest that high L2 proficiency may be a prerequisite to the cross-linguistic transfer of reading strategies.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andrews, S. (1982). Phonological recoding: Is the regularity effect consistent? Memory and Cognition, 10, 565575. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202439 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2(3), 386393. https://doi.org//10.1037//0096-1523.2.3.386 Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Bojesen Christensen, R. H., Singmann, H., Dai, B., Grothendieck, G., & Green, P. (2016). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–11. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4 Google Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Kroll, J. F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 46(6), 10221047. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000770 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Besner, D. (1990). Does the reading system need a lexicon. In D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d’Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension Processes in Reading, (pp. 7399). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Bolger, D. J., Hornickel, J., Cone, N. E., Burman, D. D., & Booth, J. R. (2008). Neural correlates of orthographic and phonological consistency effects in children. Human Brain Mapping, 29(12), 14161429. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20476 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botezatu, M. R. (in press). Does MOED rhyme with FRUIT? An event-related potential study of cross-language rhyming. Neuroreport.Google Scholar
Botezatu, M. R., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F. (in press). Proficiency in a second language influences processing of print-to-sound mappings. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism.Google Scholar
Botezatu, M. R., Peterson, S., & Garcia, D. (submitted). Consistency matters: How language experience influences spelling-sound competition dynamics in the dominant language. Frontiers in Psychology. Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Besner, D., Jonasson, J. T., & Davelaar, E. (1979). Phonological encoding in the lexical decision task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31(3), 489507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 11971211.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: Route Cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, V., & Leahy, J. (1992). Children’s and adults’ reading of nonwords: Effects of regularity and consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(4), 718.Google ScholarPubMed
Cortese, M. J., & Simpson, G. B. (2000). Regularity effects in word naming: What are they? Memory & Cognition, 28(8), 12691276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durgunoglu, A. Y., & Hancin, B. J. (1992). An overview of cross-language transfer in bilingual reading. Advances in Psychology, 83, 391411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5(4), 674691.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Misra, M., Tam, J. W., & Kroll, J. F. (2012). On the time course of accessing meaning in a second language: An electrophysiological and behavioral investigation of translation recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(5), 1165.Google Scholar
Holcomb, P. J., Grainger, J., & O’Rourke, T. (2002). An electrophysiological study of the effects of orthographic neighborhood size on printed word perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 938950. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902760191153 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holcomb, P. J., Reder, L., Misra, M., & Grainger, J. (2005). The effects of prime visibility on ERP measures of masked priming. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 155172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 106(1), 501511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, C., Tsai, J., Lee, C., & Tzeng, O. J. (2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading Chinese phonograms: An event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 108, 5666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hue, C.-W. (1992). Recognition processes in character naming. In Chen, H.-C. E. & Tzeng, O. J. L. (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese. Advances in psychology (pp. 93107). North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ide, N., & Suderman, K. (2004). The American National Corpus First Release. Language Resources and Evaluation Conference (lrec), Lisbon.Google Scholar
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 277288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.06.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jared, D. (1997). Spelling-sound consistency affects the naming of high-frequency words. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 505529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jared, D. (2002). Spelling-sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 723750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44(1), 231. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2747 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 6784). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, J., & Bishop, D. (1987). Anatomical differences between nose, palm, and foot, or, the body in question: Further dissection of the processes of sub-lexical spelling-sound translation. In Coltheart, M. (Ed.), Attention and Performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 449469). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., & Donchin, E. (1987). Brain potentials as indices of orthographic and phonological interaction during word matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 7686. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.13.1.76 Google ScholarPubMed
Kroll, J. F., Bice, K., Botezatu, M. R., & Zirnstein, M. (2022). On the dynamics of lexical access in two or more languages. In Gleitman, L., Papafragou, A., & Trueswell, J. (Eds.), Handbook of the Mental Lexicon. (pp. 583597). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacruz, I., & Folk, J. R. (2004). Feedforward and feedback consistency effects for high-and low-frequency words in lexical decision and naming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57A, 12611284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lado, R. (1957). Linguistics across cultures: Applied linguistics for language teachers. University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Lallier, M., & Carreiras, M. (2018). Cross-linguistic transfer in bilinguals reading in two alphabetic orthographies: The grain size accommodation hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 386401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, M. A. (2016). Package ‘ez’: Easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments. https://cran.r-project.org/package=ez.Google Scholar
Lee, C., Huang, H., Kuo, W., Tsai, J., & Tzeng, J. L. O. (2010). Cognitive and neural basis of the consistency and lexicality effects in reading Chinese. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 1027. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C., Tsai, J., Chan, W., Hsu, C., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An event-related potentials study. Neuroreport, 18, 147151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C., Tsai, J., Su, E. C., Tzeng, J. L. O., & Hung, D. L. (2005). Consistency, regularity, and frequency effects in naming Chinese characters. Language and Linguistics, 6, 75107.Google Scholar
Lee, C., Tsai, J. L., Huang, H. W., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. (2006). The temporal signatures of semantic and phonological activations for Chinese sublexical processing: an event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1121, 150159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, P., Zhang, F., Yu, A., & Zhao, X. (2020). Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3): An enhanced tool for assessing multilingual experience. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 938944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, I., Chen, S., & Sue, I. (2003). Regularity and consistency effects in Chinese character naming. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45, 2946. http://search.proquest.com/docview/620299941?accountid=13158 Google Scholar
Liu, I., Wu, J. T., & Chou, T. L. (1996). Encoding operation and transcoding as the major loci of the frequency effect. Cognition, 59, 149168. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00688-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2004). Mapping written input onto orthographic representations: The case of bilinguals with partially overlapping orthographies. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 26).Google Scholar
Martensen, H., Maris, E., & Dijkstra, T. (2000). When does inconsistency hurt? On the relation between phonological consistency effects and the reliability of sublexical units. Memory and Cognition, 28, 648656. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201254 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, F. H., Kaine, A., & Kirby, M. (2006). Event-related brain potentials elicited during word recognition by adult good and poor phonological decoders. Brain and Language, 96, 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.04.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703704. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1264 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meschyan, G., & Hernandez, A. E. (2006). Impact of language proficiency and orthographic transparency on bilingual word reading: An fMRI investigation. Neuroimage, 29, 11351140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 3041. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2006.00205.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F. (2012). When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(1), 224237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumtaz, S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2001). The effects of bilingualism on learning to read English: Evidence from the contrast between Urdu-English bilingual and English monolingual children. Journal of Research in Reading, 24, 113134. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.t01-1-00136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosarti, C., Mechelli, A., Green, D. W., & Price, C. J. (2010). The impact of second language learning on semantic and nonsemantic first language reading. Cerebral Cortex, 20(2), 315327. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Orthography to phonology and meaning: Comparisons across and within writing systems. Reading and Writing, 18, 193210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., Fiez, J., Nelson, J., Bolger, D. J., & Tan, L. (2007). Reading in two writing systems: Accommodation and assimilation of the brain’s reading network. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 131146. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907002891 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 101118. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.24.1.101 Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103(1), 56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychology Software Tools Incorporated. (2012). E-Prime. Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. In R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., & Castles, A. (2013). Phonological decoding or direct access? Regularity effects in lexical decisions of grade 3 and 4 children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 66, 338346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, A. I., Kroll, J. F., & Diaz, M. (2007). Reading words in Spanish and English: Mapping orthography to phonology in two languages. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(1), 106129. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960500463920 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebastian-Gallés, N., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., de Diego-Balaguer, R., & Díaz, B. (2006). First-and second-language phonological representations in the mental lexicon. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(8), 12771291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523568. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.96.4.523 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(3), 383404. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(84)90270-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K., & Posner, M. I. (1998). Establishing a time-line of word recognition: evidence from eye movements and event-related potentials. Neuroreport, 9, 21952200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143174. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 2747. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00519 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & Bauer, D. W. (1978). Experiments on the spelling-to-sound regularity effect in word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 410415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockwell, R. P., Bowen, J. D., & Martin, J. W. (1965). The grammatical structures of English and Spanish (Vol. 4). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Stone, G. O., Vanhoy, M., & Van Orden, G. C. (1997). Perception is a two-way street: Feedforward and feedback phonology in visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 337359. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.2487 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuart, M., & Masterson, J. (1992). Patterns of reading and spelling in 10-year-old children related to prereading phonological abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 168187. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(92)90034-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, M. D., Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Why is lexical retrieval slower for bilinguals? Evidence from picture naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 479488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Feng, C., Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Xiong, J., Fox, P. T., & Gao, J. (2003). Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language. Human Brain Mapping, 18, 158166. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10089 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An Event-Related Potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(02), 173204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Leerdam, M., Bosman, A. M., & de Groot, A. M. (2009). When MOOD rhymes with ROAD: Dynamics of phonological coding in bilingual visual word perception. The Mental Lexicon, 4(3), 303335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97, 6788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M. (1984). Children’s and adults’ use of spelling-sound information in three reading tasks. Memory and Cognition, 12, 293305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weekes, B. S., Castles, A. E., & Davies, R. A. (2006). Effects of consistency and age of acquisition on reading and spelling among developing readers. Reading and Writing, 19, 133169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-2032-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, W. C., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 10241037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). The Woodcock reading mastery tests—Revised. American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2004). Batería III Woodcock-Muñoz. Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Yum, Y. N., Law, S.-P., Su, I.-F., Lau, K.-Y. D., & Mo, K. N. (2014). An ERP study of effects of regularity and consistency in delayed naming and lexicality judgment in a logographic writing system. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, S., Perfetti, C. A., & Yang, H. (1999). Whole word, frequency-general phonology in semantic processing of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 858875. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.25.4.858 Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., & Braun, M. (2001). Identical words are read differently in different languages. Psychological Science, 12(5), 379384. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00370 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, J. C., Stone, G. O., & Jacobs, A. M. (1997). What is the pronunciation for –ough and the spelling for /u/? A database for computing feedforward and feedback consistency in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29, 600618. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210615 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Botezatu supplementary material

Botezatu supplementary material

Download Botezatu supplementary material(File)
File 16.4 KB