Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-02T09:49:54.436Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modeling complex interactions in a disrupted environment: Relational events in the WTC response

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 April 2023

Scott Leo Renshaw
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
Selena M. Livas
Affiliation:
University of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
Miruna G. Petrescu-Prahova
Affiliation:
University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
Carter T. Butts*
Affiliation:
University of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Email: buttsc@uci.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

When subjected to a sudden, unanticipated threat, human groups characteristically self-organize to identify the threat, determine potential responses, and act to reduce its impact. Central to this process is the challenge of coordinating information sharing and response activity within a disrupted environment. In this paper, we consider coordination in the context of responses to the 2001 World Trade Center (WTC) disaster. Using records of communications among 17 organizational units, we examine the mechanisms driving communication dynamics, with an emphasis on the emergence of coordinating roles. We employ relational event models (REMs) to identify the mechanisms shaping communications in each unit, finding a consistent pattern of behavior across units with very different characteristics. Using a simulation-based “knock-out” study, we also probe the importance of different mechanisms for hub formation. Our results suggest that, while preferential attachment and pre-disaster role structure generally contribute to the emergence of hub structure, temporally local conversational norms play a much larger role in the WTC case. We discuss broader implications for the role of microdynamics in driving macroscopic outcomes, and for the emergence of coordination in other settings.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

It is a common “disaster myth” that, when faced with a sudden threat, social groups lacking specific preparation will either passively wait for rescue, or fly into a state of uncontrolled panic (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Bevc and Kuligowski2006). In fact, it is far more common for those in harm’s way to take immediate action to assess the threat, determine appropriate response measures, and take the initiative to carry them out (Auf der Heide, Reference Auf der Heide and O’Leary2004). Such complex behavior under adverse and disrupted circumstances poses significant problems of coordination: to accomplish it, groups must compile information regarding the evolving situation (as well as relevant background knowledge), identify actions that need to be taken (and the resources needed for those actions), and direct behavior to ensure that requisite actions are performed with a minimum of task interference. Central to successful coordination is communication, the structure of which can either facilitate or inhibit performance (as has been known at least since the pioneering studies of Bavelas and colleagues (e.g., Bavelas & Barrett, Reference Bavelas and Barrett1951). At a more microdynamic level, effective communication also depends upon shared interaction norms, whether embedded in formal protocols or in conventional cultural practices, without which information passing becomes extremely difficult.

While this emergence of coordination in response to threat is well-established, the exact mechanisms by which it is accomplished in situ, and the relative importance of those mechanisms for successful response in practice, remain the subject of inquiry. Among the challenges in studying this phenomenon have been the relative paucity of detailed data on communication dynamics in disrupted settings, and until recently a lack of principled modeling strategies for inferring the driving mechanisms behind interaction processes from observational data (particularly in naturalistic settings wherein many different types of mechanisms may be simultaneously at play). Progress within the Relational Event Modeling (REM) paradigm (Butts, Reference Butts2008) has greatly lowered this latter barrier, making it practical to investigate complex microdynamics within a statistically principled framework. There remains, however, the empirical challenge of identifying and analyzing cases of emergent coordination in response to external threat.

In this paper, we contribute to this latter goal via an investigation of coordination in response to the 2001 World Trade Center (WTC) disaster. Building on the work of Butts (Reference Butts2008), who studied six small communication networks from this event, we here analyze 17 relational event systems from both specialist and non-specialist responders. Considering a range of candidate mechanisms, we identify those present in each network and estimate their effects. Further, we employ simulation-based analysis of the estimated models to probe the relative importance of different mechanisms for the emergence of hub structure—a critical coordinative adaptation in these groups. As we show, beneath the diversity of these responding groups lies considerable consistency, with the vast majority of communication mechanisms operating in similar ways across networks when present (though not all mechanisms operate in all networks). While we find that well-known mechanisms such as preferential attachment and the prominence of pre-disaster coordinative roles do consistently contribute to the formation of hub structure, a much larger fraction in fact emerges from the action of temporally local communication norms, which have the side effect of creating conversational “inertia” that leads to large differences in communication activity. The macro-level structure of the WTC communication networks is thus seen to arise in large part from microdynamic mechanisms.

The remainder of the paper proceeds as follows. First, we give a brief overview of communication in disaster context, along with the use of interpersonal radio devices (on which the present case is based) as a medium for communication. Next, we provide some details on the differences between specialist and non-specialist networks and how network size may influence communication, followed by a discussion of the mechanisms potentially involved in hub formation. We then describe our data set and the methods used for model selection, analysis, and the simulation knock-out experiment. This is followed by a presentation of the results for our REMs, as well as our simulation study. Finally, we end the paper with a discussion of the implications of our findings for communication and coordination in disrupted settings more generally, and for future studies. Our paper is also accompanied by an R package (R Core Team, 2021) with the complete WTC radio data set, thereby facilitating further analysis of this rich historical case.

2. Background

Responses to disasters depend upon a complex interplay of formal (i.e., institutional) and informal factors (Quarantelli, Reference Quarantelli and Brictson1966; Dynes, Reference Dynes1970; Stallings, Reference Stallings and Quarentelli1978). This interplay is perhaps nowhere more evident than in the domain of responder communication, where technical constraints, formal roles, and standard operating procedures must cope with responders’ shifting demands for information and the capacity to provide it. An important aspect of the total response process is the emergence of communication networks that promote information transmission and coordination. The structural characteristics of such networks, as well as the mechanisms that facilitate their formation, are of crucial importance to researchers and practitioners alike.

By definition, disasters are exceptional events, in which conventional social processes are subject to substantial disruption. While losses are perhaps the most salient characteristic of disasters, Drabek’s (Reference Drabek1986) classic synthesis of findings on disaster response emphasizes the “accidental or uncontrollable” nature of disaster events (p. 7), and the extent to which they exceed the capacity of conventional mechanisms for managing disruption. Indeed, a central feature of the US Federal Emergency Management Agency’s definition of disaster is the requirement that the event “cannot be managed through the routine procedures and resources of government” (FEMA, 1984). Uncertainty and disruption of routines are therefore an important aspect of the social responses to disaster events. Organizationally, such disruptions of routine generate a high-uncertainty environment in which coordination demands escalate, while infrastructure (both human and technical) degrades. The problem of “many people trying to do quickly what they do not ordinarily do, in an environment with which they are not familiar” (Tierney, Reference Tierney1985, p77) generates the potential for confusion and task interference, particularly where tasks are time-critical and resources are limited. Negotiating such difficulties, acquiring information about losses and ongoing hazards, and other coordinative tasks require a high degree of interpersonal communication. Thus, actors responding to a disaster depend on the emergence (or retention, if preexisting) of communication networks, which can convey information from those who have it to those who need it without placing excessive demand on the communicants (Drabek, Reference Drabek1986). While coordinative challenges are particularly acute in disasters, these kinds of issues can arise in other contexts where individuals must act rapidly in the face of a disrupted environment, or where timing is of the utmost importance (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Lomi, Mascia and Pallotti2017; Leonard & Howitt, Reference Leonard and Howitt2010; Weick, Reference Weick1993).

Also central to the nature of disaster communication is the time frame in which the communication takes place. Disaster researchers conventionally divide the “life cycle” of a disaster into several periods or phases (Fischer, Reference Fischer1998), distinguished by characteristic patterns of activities and events, e.g. a “pre-impact period” before the hazard manifests; an “impact period” during which the hazard is active; a “response period” in which damage is contained and survivors are attended to; and a “recovery period” in which attempts are made to restore conditions to a stable state. Although communication is critical in all periods, the impact and response periods constitute a crucial interval in which survivors attempt to respond to ongoing hazards, search and rescue operations begin, and emergency response organizations respond to the scene and attempt to coordinate their efforts. Within that interval, it is common in practice to refer to an “emergency phase” in which immediate, time-sensitive action is required to react to an active hazard. Communication in this period plays a vital role in facilitating situational awareness, and in coordinating response activities. At the same time, such communication is made more difficult by disruption of conventional resources, roles, and routines, the high opportunity cost of engaging in communication versus task performance, and the high cognitive load facing communicants in what is typically a confusing, fast-changing, and possibly threatening environment. Understanding the emergent dynamics of emergency phase communication thus has the potential to shed light on how groups organize in response to threats within a high-pressure setting that differs greatly from everyday conditions. To date, the lack of detailed data from real emergencies has been a major barrier to such understanding, and the methodology needed to make use of such data has only become available in recent years. In this paper, we capitalize on the unique assets of the WTC data set (described below). The 2001 WTC disaster stands as one of the largest communication-coordination “emergency phase” related events in recent history. With this data and the increasingly widely-used relational event framework, we intend to provide novel insights into communications during the emergency phase of disaster response.

2.1. Interpersonal radio communications

From the mid-20th century onward, radio communication via portable devices has been a critical tool for coordination among responders (McElroy, Reference McElroy2005), despite numerous limitations (Auf der Heide, Reference Auf der Heide1989). In the immediate post-impact period, when communications infrastructure may be degraded and alternate systems have not yet been deployed, hand-held radio devices serve to connect responders in the field to one another. In addition, the relatively low cost of hand-held transceivers in the modern context makes this technology accessible to organizations which do not specialize in emergency response activities. Since the first responders to any disaster are those who happen to be at the impact site, interpersonal radio communication is an important “workhorse” tool for improving coordination in the emergency phase of a disaster. This raises the question, however, of how responders—especially those who are not specialized in emergency response—actually use radio communication during the emergency phase of a disaster. At the most basic label, radio communications require the use of fairly rigid communication protocols to avoid confusion due to cross-talk; these are typically codified into a set of practices or standard operating procedures (SOP) that involve systematic identification of the sending party and intended receiving party, formal acknowledgments of contact and receipt, etc. In prior work on a subset of the WTC radio networks, Butts (Reference Butts2008) found strong evidence for the prominence of radio SOP via pronounced and systematic participation shift effects. Organizations using radio communications may also delegate coordination tasks to specially designated individuals (e.g. dispatchers), potentially leading to a more centralized structure in which individuals largely interact with the institutionally designated coordinator rather than directly with each other. We discuss this further below.

2.2. Specialized vs. non-specialized responders

It is commonly posited that behavioral responses to disasters by “ordinary people” within the initial phase of a hazard event are deviant and chaotic, by contrast with the disciplined and efficient actions of emergency response organizations (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Bevc and Kuligowski2006). In practice, however, organizational responses are not necessarily better coordinated than those of others on the scene (Fischer, Reference Fischer1998), and the latter may indeed prove quite effective (Auf der Heide, Reference Auf der Heide and O’Leary2004). As Fischer (Reference Fischer1998) famously observed, variation in extent of prior planning, rehearsal of plans, and previous experience with similar events plays a large role in determining which organizational responses are successful and which fall short (a point also noted by e.g. Drabek, Reference Drabek1986; Auf der Heide, Reference Auf der Heide1989). That said, since the first individuals and organizations to respond to a disaster are those who happen to be present when the impact occurs, it is not necessarily the case that the true “first responders” will be trained for or equipped to deal with the event at hand. Much emergency-phase response activity is thus improvised, but it does not follow that such activity will be disorganized or ineffective (Wachtendorf, Reference Wachtendorf2004; Mendonça et al., Reference Mendonça, Webb, Butts and Brooks2014). Repurposing of existing communication networks, together with the emergence of new ones, may result in highly structured interaction patterns. The question of how such networks develop, then, and their dependence upon pre-disaster organization, is of clear importance to understanding responder communication in the immediate post-impact period.

In the case of radio communications by teams of WTC responders, Butts et al. (Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007) distinguish between groups of “specialist” responders—police, security, or other personnel who are specifically trained and organized to respond to emergencies (if not disasters)—and “non-specialist” responders (e.g., maintenance personnel) who are present and active at the scene, but whose conventional organizational structure and practices are not intended for emergency response. Specialist and non-specialist groups responding to a disaster share goals such as getting individuals to safety (Quarantelli, Reference Quarantelli1960; Mileti et al., Reference Mileti, Drabek and Haas1975; Abe, Reference Abe1976; Noji, Reference Noji and Noji1997), but specialist groups may also be charged with other objectives (e.g., securing the area, taking actions to prevent or mitigate emerging or ongoing hazards, coordinating with other organizational units, etc.) that may pose different communicative or coordinative challenges. Butts et al. (Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007) found that the time-aggregated networks of communication among specialist and non-specialist groups share structural features, with relatively minor quantitative differences but a high level of overall similarity. In a dynamic analysis of the six smallest WTC networks, Butts (Reference Butts2008) also found broad similarity in the communication patterns of specialist and non-specialist responders. However, some consistent differences have also been identified; for instance, an analysis of robustness of the WTC networks to attack (Fitzhugh & Butts, Reference Fitzhugh and Butts2021) found that specialists were consistently more dependent upon individuals in institutionalized coordinative roles to maintain connectivity (and hence more vulnerable to their removal). This last suggests a potential difference between specialist and non-specialist networks in their hub organization, a matter that we revisit in our simulation study below.

2.3. Emergent coordination and institutionalized coordinative roles

In cross-sectional analyses of the WTC data, Petrescu-Prahova & Butts (Reference Petrescu-Prahova and Butts2008) show evidence that both specialist and non-specialist networks are held together by a relatively small number of highly central actors (coordinators). While some of these actors occupy institutionalized coordinative roles (e.g., manager, dispatcher) that formalize their status as a coordinator under nominal conditions, the relationship between such formal roles and actual coordination is imperfect. Although individuals occupying an institutionalized coordinator role (ICR) are more likely to become coordinators than those without such roles, the majority of coordinators were found to be emergent (i.e., to lack an ICR). This raises the question of what drives the emergence of coordination during the unfolding emergency, and the relationship of those drivers to ICR status. Using REMs, we can directly probe the effect of ICR occupancy on communication behavior patterns, as well as differences in these behaviors between specialist and non-specialist networks (an effect not examined in previous work). Using simulation, we can further analyze the impact of ICR effects on the formation of communication hubs at the network level, allowing us to investigate the extent to which behavioral differences involving ICRs do or do not ramify into differences in structural position (and to which this varies between specialist and non-specialist networks).

2.4. Network size

Another factor that may plausibly impact WTC radio communications is network size. The 17 networks vary over a large range, from 24 to 256 individuals (mean 127), reflecting substantial differences in the complexity of events within the group of actors and the cognitive load entailed in keeping track of events within the group. This may lead to differences in the relevant mechanisms driving communication between networks; for instance, egocentric “tracking” of distinct communication threads beyond the currently active conversation is likely to be of substantially greater importance in large networks with many different lines of activity, while adherence to local conversational norms may be greater in smaller groups with fewer risks of interruption. Likewise, some mechanisms may have greater impact in networks of larger or smaller size. For instance, preferential attachment may have a greater impact in larger networks, as there may be less a priori clarity in who is available to communicate in a large group and a correspondingly greater signal value of visibility. For ICRs, the task of coordinating activity becomes more difficult in larger networks, as they must coordinate a larger number of actors as well as relay information to more individuals, while presumably receiving information from more sources as well. This may plausibly reduce the gap between ICRs and other members of the network, as emergent coordinators step in to perform tasks that ICRs cannot. Below, we examine the question of whether these and other mechanisms operate differently in networks of different size.

3. Potential hub-forming mechanisms

A consistent finding in prior descriptive analyses of the WTC networks (Butts et al., Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007; Petrescu-Prahova & Butts, Reference Petrescu-Prahova and Butts2008; Fitzhugh & Butts, Reference Fitzhugh and Butts2021) is that specialist and non-specialist networks alike are held together by a relatively small number of individuals occupying hub-like coordinator roles. These positions are distinguished by high degree, betweenness, and total communication volume, and are reflective of individuals who played outsized roles in coordinating activity during the WTC event. As noted above, the majority of these positions are emergent, in that that do not correspond to ICR membership, although those in ICRs are disproportionately likely to occupy coordinator/hub roles. This raises the question of whence these hubs come: what are the dynamic mechanisms that drive the hub-focused organization of the WTC networks, and are these mechanisms consistent across organizational units?Footnote 1 Here, we consider three categories of mechanisms that could plausibly account—individually, or in tandem—for hub formation in the WTC radio networks.

3.1. Preferential attachment

One of the best known mechanisms driving the emergence of hubs in social networks is preferential attachment (Price, Reference Price1976). Early empirical studies of preferential attachment go back as far as the 1950s and 1960s (Merton, Reference Merton1968; Simon, Reference Simon1955), with the emphasis on “cumulative advantage” processes in settings such as citation networks, where new entrants tend to cite papers already cited by others; the derivation of power law degree distributions from such processes is due to Price (Reference Price1976), with much later rediscoveries several decades later by researchers outside the social network community. In the context of a relational event process with fixed vertices, preferential attachment can be understood as an increasing propensity to direct events towards vertices with a larger share of prior communication (i.e., larger total communication volume), as implemented e.g. by Butts (Reference Butts2008), Gibson et al. (Reference Gibson, Buchler, Hoffman and La Fleur2019, Reference Gibson, Hoffman, Fleur and Buchler2021). In a radio communication setting, those who speak first are known to all persons attending to the channel to be active, thus making them a likely target for incoming communications. This may in turn lead to their receiving more calls, their responding and thus getting more air time. Over time, this process may produce a positive feedback loop in which those who are active early on (possibly for idiosyncratic reasons) end up becoming hubs. In the case of the WTC disaster, there are reasons why we might expect this pattern to appear. First, communication is occurring through the use of radio, meaning the first responders cannot see who they are communicating to, or identify who is available to communicate with. The simplest way to know who is available to respond is by noting who has already spoken, making the likelihood of their response high. Secondly, in the early stages of the disaster, individuals are cognitively overloaded and often disoriented; the cognitive load can be eased by choosing to communicate to those who are known to be already talking rather than trying to call individuals who may not be able to respond. Notably, this does not require individuals to store and recall the detailed record of past communications, but only to maintain a general sense of relative frequency. This type of mnemonic task is relatively undemanding and can often be performed where detailed event memory is infeasible (Sudman et al., Reference Sudman, Bradburn and Schwarz1996).

The social mechanism of preferential attachment may also be more likely to drive the formation of hubs in non-specialist networks when compared to their specialist counterparts. Specialist networks have protocols in place that often dictate a pre-planned chain of command for whom to contact in the case of a disaster, which may make them less likely to experience preferential attachment. Non-specialists lack these preexisting norms and training, leading to a greater reliance on the “call whom you hear” heuristic that drives preferential attachment.

3.2 Conversational inertia

A second mechanism that could account for the emergence of coordination hubs is that of conversational inertia, which arises from the turn-taking structure of conversational norms (in this case, radio SOP). Parties engaged in conversation tend to remain in conversation (hence the term inertia), either because of the back-and-forth of call/response dynamics, or because of other normative actions such as sequential address (in which the same party continues to be the sender), baton-passing (in which the receiving party becomes the sender), “piling on” (in which the current receiving party becomes the subject of additional incoming communications), etc. tend to keep one or both currently communicating parties involved. This produces a form of autocorrelation that can potentially amplify other sources of variation (including random events) in communication volume: individuals who, for whatever reason, are drawn into conversation tend to stay there, accumulating substantially more communication volume than those who are not.Footnote 2 As we shall show, this impact turns out to be substantially greater than might be supposed.

3.3. Institutionalized Coordinator Roles (ICR)

The last hub forming mechanism we investigate here is differential behavior by and towards individuals occupying institutionalized coordinative roles. As discussed above, persons in ICRs (whom we simply refer to as “ICRs” where there is no danger of confusion) are expected, to receiver and relay information, direct action, and perform other duties requiring high levels of communication. ICRs have also been found to be more likely to occupy coordinator roles in practice (Petrescu-Prahova & Butts, Reference Petrescu-Prahova and Butts2008), though as noted above most hubs are not ICRs. Similar results have been seen in other contexts, with those individuals in superordinate roles or rank in conversational contexts tending to talk more (Fisek et al., Reference Fisek, Berger and Norman1991) and higher ranking individuals being more likely to send and receive e-mail communications (Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Buchler, Hoffman and La Fleur2019). It is thus highly plausible that ICRs are more attractive targets for interaction, and more active in initiating contact with others, thereby contributing to hub formation. This may be particularly important in specialist networks, where centralizing coordination activity on a small number of pre-assigned hub roles is often a deliberate strategy (albeit one that may fail in the context of a real disaster).

4. Data and methods

4.1. Data

The data we employ here are derived from transcripts of radio communications among responders to the WTC disaster on the morning of November 9, 2001, released by the Port Authority of New York and New Jersey and coded by Butts et al. (Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007). We analyze the radio communication transcripts from seventeen groups of responders to the WTC disaster, each of whom was using one radio channel exclusively. All transcripts begin immediately after the first airplane crashed into the North Tower at 8:46 am and extend for 3 hours and 33 minutes or until communication was terminated by structural collapse (as occurred for some units located within the WTC complex).

Based on information provided by the original transcriber and/or other transcript content, a unique identifier was assigned to the sender and named target(s) of each transmission (Butts et al., Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007). Where one-to-many communications were encountered, each was coded as a series of dyadic transmissions from the sender to each of the named recipients (in the order named). Transmissions with no clear target(s), and/or targets that were identified only as a group (e.g., “anyone,” “all units”) were not included. The resulting lists of ordered transmissions (one per transcript) comprised the relational event sets employed in subsequent analyses. Lengths range from 70 to 1,145 eligible transmissions, with the number of named communicants ranging from 24 to 256. Figure 1 illustrates the networks in question, while showcasing the coordinative hub structure we aim to investigate. In conjunction with the relational event data itself, we consider the formal coordinative status of individual responders as an illustrative covariate and distinguish between specialist and non-specialist responder networks, using the classification criteria provided by Butts et al. (Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007). A package containing the full data set, with additional details on data structure and coding is included as a supplement to this paper.

Figure 1. Visualization of the time-marginalized WTC networks, sorted by specialization. All networks show high levels of centralization, with a relatively small number of coordinators providing much of the connectivity in each case.

Summary statistics for the 17 networks are shown in Table 1, including the number of individuals in the system (network size), the number of events, the fraction of individuals who are ICRs, and the specialist/non-specialist encoding. About 6.5% of responders occupy ICRs, with this fraction varying from 1.2% to 16%. Butts et al. (Reference Butts, Petrescu-Prahova and Cross2007) previously found that these networks are highly centralized, as is evident in Figure 1.

Table 1. Summary statistics for the WTC radio networks

4.2. Methods

4.2.1. Relational event models

As in the original work by Butts (Reference Butts2008), we use the relational event modeling framework to understand the effects of various conversational drivers involved in the formation of emergency communication networks. We specifically use ordinal REMs, as this data was originally coded from time-ordered radio transcripts without exact timestamps. Because the 17 networks studied here vary greatly in size, specialization, location, and types of task performance, it is entirely plausible that different mechanisms are active in different groups; rather than attempting to fit a pooled model (or a hierarchical model with common effects, as in DuBois et al., Reference DuBois, Butts, McFarland and Smyth2013a), we instead proceed by (1) identifying a space of substantively plausible models, and then (2) performing complexity-penalized model selection (AICc) to find the effects active in each network. We verify adequacy of the selected models using local prediction and recall. (All models were fit and simulations performed using the relevent package Butts, Reference Butts2013 v1.1 for the R statistical computing system R Core Team, 2021.)

The space of plausible mechanisms includes those discussed by Butts (Reference Butts2008), as well as effects motivated by the background discussion above. The effects considered include the following. Preferential attachment, where ego tends to call those with more airtime, is parameterized as the effect of normalized total communication volume on call receipt (NTDegRec). This results in a single statistic whose parameter will be positive if preferential attachment is present and negative if a rotating turn effect is present. Next, we consider cognitive effects: persistence (previous out-alters are salient for ego’s out-calls) is parameterized as the tendency for responders to direct calls to those that they have previously directed calls to, and recency (more recent in-alters are salient for ego’s out-calls) is parameterized as the tendency for responders to direct calls to those that they have most recently received calls from (recency of receipt Rr and recency of sending Rs). A positive value for these parameters would indicate the presence of these cognitive mechanisms, while negative values might indicate a search pattern for individuals or perhaps an attempt to disseminate information across a wide swath of the network.

Similar to triadic effects in the static networks, there are potential patterns of triadic closure in the REM framework. For the first two of these statistics, one is parameterized as the number of outbound two paths from ego to some alter and the other as the number of inbound two paths from some alter to ego (OTPSnd and ITPSnd). For the former, if the parameter is positive, this indicates a pattern of transitive closure. Substantively, this may come from responders choosing to share information with individuals directly, rather than through third parties. If this parameter is negative, it may indicate that responders are choosing to rely on intermediaries to spread information throughout the network. For the latter, a positive coefficient for this statistic would indicate a tendency toward cyclic closure. This would indicate that if individuals are relaying information through intermediaries, those on the receiving end have a tendency to reply directly. A negative coefficient would indicate a tendency away from cycles and imply that individuals more often respond to the intermediaries, rather than those two steps away. We also include out-bound and incoming shared partner effects OSPSnd and ISPSnd—analogous to the sibling effects of Fararo & Sunshine (Reference Fararo and Sunshine1964)—to help understand other triadic communication patterns between communicants.

The next set of parameters are conversational inertia terms or participation shifts (tendencies reflecting “local” conversational norms (from Gibson, Reference Gibson2003). Dyadic participation shifts (p-shifts) are conventionally described by the ordered pair AB (denoting the respective sender and receiver), followed by a second pair indicating the sender and receiver of the next event. An AB-BA shift thus involves an immediate response of B to a call from A, an AB-BY shift involves the recipient of a call immediately calling a third party, etc. The shifts included in the present analysis are PSAB-BA or call and response, PSAB-AY and PSAB-XB or persistence of source and target, respectively, PSAB-XA or source attraction, and finally PSAB-BY or “handing off” of communication. For a more in-depth discussion on participation shifts, please see Gibson (Reference Gibson2003). Lastly, the pre-disaster organizational role is parameterized as a covariate effect for ego occupying and institutional coordinative role (ICR) on in and out calls.

To estimate model parameters, we perform Bayesian inference using the Laplace approximation on all 17 WTC networks, using diffuse $t$ -priors with prior location 0, scale 10, and 4 degrees of freedom; model selection is performed by AICc minimization, as described below. Comparison of parameters from fitted models is used to assess the differences between specialist and non-specialist responders, similar versus dissimilar mechanisms driving behavior across networks, and to evaluate the respective impact of ICR membership and endogenous factors in shaping the emergence of coordinator roles.

4.2.2. Model selection

We begin by noting that it is plausible that different mechanisms may be active in different networks, and that—recognizing that any model for a complex system is at best an approximation, rather than a “true” model—we focus on identifying collections of mechanisms likely to have good generalization performance (in terms of deviance under hypothetical replication). This motivates the use of model selection via the (sample-size corrected) Akaike’s Information Criterion (AICc), which is a complexity-adjusted estimator of replicate deviance (Akaike, Reference Akaike1974; Bozdogan, Reference Bozdogan2000). Given the set of candidate terms, we attempt to select the model that minimizes the AICc (a procedure analogous to so-called “ $L_0$ ” regularized model selection). This is a non-convex optimization problem, requiring exhaustive enumeration for an exact solution. We thus approximate the exact solution by a local optimization procedure (aka “hill-climbing”) that seeks the AICc-optimal model by steepest descent. At each iteration, the procedure considers all single-term changes to the current model (addition or deletion of terms), taking the change that results in the greatest AICc reduction and terminating when no improvement can be made. We initialize the search procedure with the empty (null) model. As a check on the quality of the hill climbing optimization process, we generated a full factorial design using the smallest network, PATH Radio Communications, which was small enough for enumerative search; the hill-climbing procedure was indeed able to find the optimal model in this case. Manual inspection of paths followed by the search procedure likewise showed no evidence of pathological behavior. While—as with any heuristic optimization procedure—it is not possible to guarantee that the optimal model is identified in every case, this approach does guarantee that (1) the selected model is locally optimal (i.e., it cannot be improved by adding or removing effects), and (2) if not a null or one-term model, the selected model is better than the null or any one-term model.

4.2.3. Knock-out simulation experiment

To better understand how preferential attachment, ICR, and conversational inertia terms influence the hub-generation process in these radio communication networks, we perform a series of computational “knock-out” experiments for these effects. In a knock-out experiment, one probes the behavior of a fixed system by removing or disabling a component (while leaving all other aspects fixed), and observing the impact of the removal on the system’s functioning. Commonly leveraged in the biological sciences, a knock-out study might, for example, investigate whether a particular enzyme was critical to mouse vision by preventing is expression or blocking its activity and assessing the mouse’s visual acuity after treatment (Serra et al., Reference Serra, Villani and Semeria2004; Laird et al., Reference Laird, Gardner, Kopel, Kerov, Lee and Baker2019). Loss of acuity, or lack thereof, provides information on the necessity of the enzyme to vision. In this study, we probe the importance of target mechanisms within our empirically-calibrated models by removing them (leaving all other aspects of the model intact) and observing the resulting change in hub formation.Footnote 3 (Rørth et al., Reference Rørth, Szabo, Bailey, Laverty, Rehm, Rubin and Cohen1998). If e.g., preferential attachment uniquely drives hub formation within a given process model, then “turning it off” will result in a substantially less hub-dominated network. Unlike lab mice, we obviously cannot carry out such experiments on the organizations responding to the WTC disaster; we can, however, perform an in silico analog using our models of the respective communication processes. Thus, in our study, the knock-out simulation serves to probe the role of PA, ICR, and conversational inertia hub formation in the WTC networks.

To conduct the “knock-out” experiments for our study, we use the simulate function from the relevent package to take draws from the posterior predictive distribution of relational event trajectories from each network, drawing 50 trajectories of equal length to the observed data (and with identical covariates) with parameters simulated using the fitted posterior mean and variance–covariance matrix. (We employ the Laplace approximation, treating the posterior as multivariate Gaussian.)

We then conduct 50 de novo sequence simulations for each of the four knock-out conditions: in the first we remove (set to 0) the preferential attachment term (NTDegRec); in the second we remove conversational inertia terms (e.g., PSAB-BA, PSAB-BY etc.); in the third we remove the ICR term; and the final condition all of the three hub-forming term sets are removed. All non-removed parameters in each condition take the same values as were used for the full model. This process results in a total of 5,100 simulated trajectories across the 17 networks.

In order to examine the impact of mechanisms on the generalized tendency to form hubs, we need a way to characterize how concentrated the distribution of the communication volume is. We choose a widely used concentration index, the Theil index (shown in Equation (1)) (Theil, Reference Theil1967) for this purpose, which is has been used to study various phenomena: from income inequality (Silva & Leichenko, Reference Silva and Leichenko2009), crime (Kang, Reference Kang2016), to health disparities (Borrell & Talih, Reference Borrell and Talih2011; Manz & Mansmann Reference Manz and Mansmann2021), just to name a few.

(1) \begin{equation} T_T=\frac{1}{N}\sum _{i=1}^{N} \frac{x_i}{\mu } \ln \left ( \frac{x_i}{\mu } \right ) \end{equation}

It has a natural interpretation in terms of the entropic “cost” of going from an equal distribution to that of the observed system—we can think of the Theil index as expressing the extent to which social mechanisms are systematically organizing/biasing activity, versus letting it happen at random, making it a sensible measure of communication volume concentration in our networks.

Using the simulations in each condition, we then calculate the Theil index for each network using total communication volume per actor (taking the mean index value over all simulated replicates). We do not treat sending and receiving separately, as sending and receiving volumes have a median correlation of approximately 0.96. We assess the contribution of each mechanism to the extent of hub formation by examining the reduction in the mean Theil index when the respective set of terms is removed (versus the full model). Larger reductions imply a greater role for the associated mechanism in hub formation in the respective network. (For cases in which a given mechanism was not in the best fitting model, its contribution is trivially 0.)

Table 2. Model adequacy checks. Observed and null probabilities of matching features of the next event in each sequence (“Eiher” implies that sender or receiver match, while “All” implies that both sender and receiver match). All models correctly identify the next event with probability greatly exceeding the null model, on average doing so the majority of the time. Recall columns show the fraction of observed events covered by the respective fraction of probability-ordered predictions (higher is better)

5. Results

5.1. Model adequacy

In order to check the adequacy of our models, we assess the ability of each model to accurately identify the next event in its respective event history. Table 2 shows the observed probability that the model was able to accurately predict either sender or receiver for a given event, followed by the null random probability of predicting either sender or receiver, then the observed probability of the model correctly guessing both the sender and receiver of a given event, and finally, its corresponding null random choice probability. Despite the seemingly chaotic communication environment of the WTC, the models perform extremely well. On average across all models, our ability to predict either of the two individuals in a given communication event correctly is 68%, meaning that in approximately 68% of cases the model’s top choice for the next event in the sequence specifies the sender or receiver correctly. The lowest prediction probability for a specific model is for the largest network of the set, Lincoln Tunnel, and even in this case the model is able to predict either the sender or receiver from the set of 229 individuals 23% of the time. This is substantially larger than the probability of correct prediction under the null model. We also examine the probability of correctly predicting both sender and receiver, an extremely difficult task. On average, we find that our models can accurately predict both sender and receiver for the next event 58% of the time, with for some models as high as 78% and for the Lincoln Tunnel model, 8% of the time. While the latter seems low in absolute terms, we observe that the likelihood of randomly guessing the sender and receiver combination for Lincoln Tunnel is 0.000002%, with 10 other networks also having random predictions correct at a similarly low probability. While getting the next event right is a very strong test of adequacy, we also consider more general recall rates, i.e., the fraction of observed events that are “covered” by the top $k$ % of predictions (the events judged most likely to occur). We examine recall rates for the top 1%, 5%, and 10% of predictions, providing a sense of the ability of the model to focus attention on events that are relatively likely to occur. As Table 2 shows, the vast majority of events are covered by the top few percent of predictions, with 76% on average contained within the top 1%, and over 90% within the top 10%. These results suggest that the selected models are able to capture the dynamics of the WTC system with sufficient fidelity to advance to the next stage of analysis.

In addition to verifying that the fitted models can locally reproduce the event history, we also verify that they can produce levels of hub formation that are compatible with the observed WTC networks. For this, we simulate complete (entirely de novo) event sequences from each model, having the same length as the original, and compute the Theil index for the total communication volume. Figure 2 shows the resulting 95% simulation intervals, along with the observed values. 14 out of 17 networks have observed Theil index values within the 95% simulation interval, indicating reasonable overall coverage, and all models produce high levels of concentration in communication volume. We thus consider the selected models to be suitable for further study.

Figure 2. The 95% simulation intervals of the Theil index of communication volume for all AICc-selected models, by network. Grey boxes display the inner quartile range, while the whiskers span the 0.025 to 97.5 percentiles. Mean value is shown in red and observed Theil index value is shown in blue.

5.2. Relational event model analysis

The coefficients for the selected models (posterior mean estimates) are shown in Tables 34; 95% posterior intervals can also be seen in Figure 3. For convenience in identifying effects whose signs are well-determined by the data, Tables 3 and 4 identify posterior mean estimates for which the central 95%, 99%, and 99.9% posterior intervals (sometimes called “credible intervals”) exclude 0.

Figure 3. Posterior modes and their asymptotic 95% posterior intervals for the event model parameters for all 17 WTC radio communication networks. Darker colored line segments represent specialist networks, while lightly colored line segments represent non-specialist networks. Term codes are as follows: PA, preferential attachment; P, persistence; Rr, recency of receipt; Rs, recency of sending; ICR, role effect; T-OTP, outgoing two-path; I-ITP, incoming two-path; T-ISP, incoming shared partners; T-OSP, outgoing shared partners; PS- $^{*}$ , p-shift effects.

Table 3. Posterior means and standard deviations for AICc-selected models for the specialist networks

Stars indicate 0 excluded from posterior intervals: 95%: $^*$ , 99%: $^{**}$ , 99.9%: $^{***}$

Table 4. Posterior means and standard deviations for AICc-selected models for the non-specialist networks

Stars indicate 0 Excluded from posterior intervals: 95%: $^*$ , 99%: $^{**}$ , 99.9%: $^{***}$

To summarize the mechanisms active in each network, Table 5 provides a schematic view of effects across networks, with their estimated signs. One of the more striking results is that all network final models contain the turn taking p-shift term, PSAB-BA, meaning that in all sizes of networks and regardless of specialization, turn taking is an important feature in driving the local patterns interaction. Additionally, the PSAB-BA term is the only one present in all final models. Some other commonly occurring terms that do not relate to the hub formation process, but that modulate the overall communication process and control for communicational differences between networks include: recency in receiving (RRecSnd), present in 88.2% of all networks, recency in sending (RSndSnd) occurring in 88.2% of all networks, persistence (FrPSndSnd), which is in 70.6% of all networks, and the outbound two path term (OTPSnd), present in 58.8% of all final networks. Other effects, like OSPSnd (present in 47% of all networks), ITPnd (present in 35.3% of all networks), and ISPnd (present in 29.4% of all networks), are less common and have little effect.

Table 5. Direction for included effects, selected models. Positive coefficients are shown in green, negative in orange; effects not included are blank. Most mechanisms show consistent effects across the networks in which they are present

RRecSnd is a term for recency of receiving a communication from an alter affecting the actor’s current rate of sending to that particular alter, while RSndSnd is the recency of sending a communication to an alter affecting the actor’s rate of sending to that same alter. For the networks in which they are present, the mean value is 1.82 for RRecSnd and 2.63 for RSndSnd. Both effects are always positive when they appear in a final model. This would suggest that these cognitive mechanisms do govern some of the observed conversational patterns, with individuals relying on recall when taking an action to address individuals in the network.

FrPSndSnd is a general effect for persistence, which is the fraction of an actor’s past sending actions to a particular alter affecting their future rate of sending to that alter; this effect is always negative when present, with a maximum value of –0.50, a minimum value of –2.88, and a mean value of –1.36. This term being negative suggests an overall movement of conversation, rather than individuals continuously addressing only a small proportion of the network.

OTPSnd and ITPSnd are both triadic two-path effects, measuring the effect of the number of out-bound or incoming two-paths from actor to alter or alter to actor on the rate of actors sending to that alter. OTPSnd ranges from –0.12 to 0.52, with a mean of 0.15, while ITPSnd ranges from –0.35 to 0.60, with a mean value of 0.03. This suggests that networks vary in their three-way communication styles, with some likely to observe triadic closure, while others trend away from these patterns. OSPSnd and ISPSnd are triadic terms for out-bound and incoming shared partner effects, where the number of shared partners between actor and alter affect the future rate of sending to that alter. OSPSnd ranges from –0.38 to 0.21, with a mean of –0.01. ISPSnd has a range between –0.30 and 0.22, with a mean at 0.01, which again suggests wide variation in triadic communication structure.

5.3. Hub-forming mechanisms

Figure 3 provides a visual representation of the posterior modes and their asymptotic 95% posterior intervals for the event model parameters for each of the 17 networks. Line segments that are darkly colored represent the specialist networks, while the lightly colored lines represent their non-specialist counterparts. Despite some small differences between specialists and non-specialists, in particular their slight difference in magnitude for preferential attachment, the effects are overwhelmingly similar between the two groups. Effects are especially similar in regards to direction, and in the majority of cases the magnitude of the effects are also strikingly similar. This can also be seen in Table 5, which clearly shows strong similarities in patterns across networks. Indeed, Figure 3 suggests that some effects may be even more consistent than Table 5 would indicate, since some apparently reverse-signed terms have relatively uncertain signs (as evidenced by 95% posterior intervals that include 0).

Overall it appears that, when the respective mechanism is active in a given network, recency on receiving, recency on sending, and all of five conversational inertia terms (PSAB-BA, PSAB-BY, PSAB-XA, PSAB-XB, PSAB-AY) enhance event probability. Likewise, persistence always has a negative influence. ICR and preferential attachment effects are nearly always positive (with the former having fairly similar size), but some networks show negative modal estimates (though the 95% PIs are wide and cross zero, suggesting that the direction of effect is somewhat uncertain). We observe that preferential attachment effects tend to be hard to estimate, with large posterior uncertainties, making quantitative comparisons across cases difficult. However, the overwhelming majority of preferential attachment effects are positive, with two networks as possible exceptions (though their 95% posterior intervals include positive values). The triadic structure terms (OTP, ITP, OSP, ISP) seem to have small magnitude effects of varying direction, and in some cases are of uncertain sign. Their persistent inclusion and predominantly positive effects indicate systematic tendencies towards triadic closure, but it is possible that these reflect relatively general pressure for cohesive interaction that is not greatly sensitive to the type of triadic structure involved.

Considering these effects in more detail, we find that for the conversational inertia terms have the following positive effects on event probability: PSAB-BA, call and response, the most ubiquitous term found in every optimized final model, on average has an effect of 6.8, a minimum of 2.9 and a maximum effect of 9.9. PSAB-XA, source attraction, found in all but one model, has an effect mean of 3, a min of 0, and a max of 3.63. The PSAB-BY, “handing off” term, is also found in all but one model, with a mean effect of 2.4, a min of 0, and a maximum value of 3.9. The fourth participation shift effect, found in 88% of all models, is the PSAB-AY, sequential address term. Sequential address has a mean effect of 2.4, a min effect of 0, and a maximum value of 4. Finally, PSAB-XB, the effect of turn usurping, is found in 70% of the models and accounts for a mean effect of 1.4, a min of 0, and a maximum of 2.9.

The effect of preferential attachment was found to be in 82% of the optimized final models. The mean effect for preferential attachment, where present, has a mean effect of 3.5, with a maximum effect of 11.4.

Figure 4. Excess concentration in communication volume (above no-hub mechanism baseline) as a function of knock-out condition; values of 1.0 correspond to full model. Networks differ in importance of ICR and PA effects for hub formation, while removing p-shift effects nearly always greatly reduces concentration. (Note: concentration outside the 0-1 interval is possible.).

The covariate effect for receiving and sending by ICRs appears in 65% of all networks, with a minimum effect of –0.67, a mean effect of 0.52, and maximum effect of 1.7. Because of the way this term is specified, if two individuals of ICR status in the network communicate to one another the event probability effect is doubled: for instance, in the case of PATH police which has the greatest effect for ICR, the effect for two ICRs communicating would be $1.7\times 2 = 3.4$ . Those networks with high-certainty coefficients have positive effects, but we see two cases with small negative posterior means and posterior intervals that place considerable mass in the positive direction.

Finally, our analyses showed no systematic differences between the average coefficient of specialized and non-specialized networks. Therefore, at least in terms of effect magnitude and direction, specialized and non-specialized networks during a disaster event are more similar than they are different, despite what we might otherwise expect.

5.4. Knock-out experiment results

Our initial analysis suggests that the three hub-forming covariates of preferential attachment, ICR, and conversational inertia can all matter in terms of predicting the local conversational patterns, but does not directly address how these mechanisms contribute to hub formation (an emergent, macroscopic outcome of social microdynamics). Here we employ simulation to investigate this latter question through use of in silico knock-out experiments—allowing us to probe the extent to which removing each mechanism suppresses (or enhances) hub formation.

Figure 4 provides an intuitive visual representation of the results from simulations of each network (the corresponding Theil index values can be found in Table 6). Here, we define the “baseline” concentration level when no hub effects are included as 0 (no “excess” concentration), normalizing all concentration values by affine transformation so that the level of concentration when all hub effects are included is set equal to 1 (100% of the “excess” concentration produced by all effects in tandem). Given this scale, we can now compare across networks the relative amount of excess concentration that remains when we suppress each class of hub effect (vertical bars). Note that while the fraction of concentration observed after knocking out an effect is usually in the [0, 1] interval, it does not have to be: values higher than the “all hub effects” level, or lower than the “no hub effects” level, can occur when either (1) a nominally hub-promoting mechanism actually inhibits hub formation in a specific network, or (2) nonlinear interactions between dynamic mechanisms lead to nonmonotonic outcomes. The most obvious cases here involve the two networks (Lincoln Tunnel Police and Newark Police) with negative preferential attachment effects; since the PA mechanism is on average hub suppressive for these two cases, knocking it out actually enhances hub formation. For convenience in interpretation, networks in Figure 4 are ordered using a 1-dimensional non-metric multidimensional scaling of the Euclidean distance between their respective concentration levels; this highlights both similarities and differences across networks. Percentage change in values following knock-out can be found in Table 7.

Table 6. Mean Theil index before and after mechanism knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms; for removed terms, PA = preferential attachment, PS = p-shifts, ICR = ICR covariate, all=all hub-forming mechanisms

Overall, conversational inertia terms account for the largest individual factor, reducing the Theil index when they are systematically removed from the models, with these terms comprising the strongest hub-forming mechanism in 88% of the networks. Knocking them out reduces hub formation in all of the networks we analyzed; by removing participation shifts, there was a 76.6% reduction on average. In supplemental analyses (see Appendix), we further consider the separate impact of each p-shift mechanism on concentration. These experiments suggest that the AB-BA shift is responsible in nearly all cases for the bulk of the hub formation effect, with other shifts playing a significant role in specific cases. Thus, a great deal of the centralization within the WTC networks appears to emerge from simple reciprocal turn-taking.

Referring to Figure 4, for one network, Newark Police, we find that by removing the conversational inertia, p-shift, terms while keeping preferential attachment, that the network has a 110% reduction in the Theil index—thus we are seeing a 10% greater reduction for just removing p-shifts than a model with none of the hub-forming terms. (This appears to be driven, as noted, by the presence of a hub-suppressive PA effect.)

Table 7. Percentage change in Theil index of communication volume mechanism knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms; for removed terms, PA = preferential attachment, PS = p-shifts, ICR = ICR covariate, all=all hub-forming mechanisms. $p$ -values reflect two-sample $t$ -tests (knock-out vs. full model)

$^{*}$ $p\lt 0.05$ ; $^{**}$ $p\lt 0.01$ ; $^{***}$ $p\lt 0.001$

Knocking out ICR effects in these networks tends also to reliably reduce hub-formation, with this effect being significantly negative in 73% of the networks the effect appears in. Across all networks, where the ICR term was eligible to be removed, we found a mean reduction of 40% in the Theil index. Referring to Figure 4, for PATH Police and Lincoln Tunnel Police, we find that ICR removal provides a larger reduction in hub-formation than conversational inertia term’s effect, at 91% and 97%, respectively.

Preferential attachment (PA) follows suit with conversational inertia and ICR effects, where 78% of the models, where preferential attachment was removed, caused a significant reduction in the Theil index of inequality of total communication volume. The average effect of removing PA, in the networks it is present in, is a 32% reduction in the Theil index for communication volume, with the largest reduction in the Newark Operations Terminals at 84%. However, as noted above, for Lincoln Tunnel Police and Newark Police, we find that by removing the PA term the resulting models have increases in the Theil index, of 7.5% and 13%. Therefore, it appears that on average conversational inertia terms (participation shifts) have the largest impact on system level hub formation (76%), followed by ICRs (40%), and preferential attachment (32%).

Looking across the effects in Figure 4, we observe that the impact of p-shifts is consistent, but that there is a general trend in which ICR effects trade off against PA effects: networks that see large reductions in hub formation when ICR effects are knocked out show smaller effects for knocking out PA effects, and vice versa. This suggests a mechanism for the contrast between ICRs and emergent coordinators discussed by Petrescu-Prahova & Butts (Reference Petrescu-Prahova and Butts2008), with preferential attachment arising as a heuristic for filling coordinator roles when ICRs are unable to carry the requisite load. It should be noted in this regard that for many of these networks, positive effects for both ICR interaction and preferential attachment are present; clearly, however, the mechanisms are not equally critical to hub formation in all networks.

Though we do see some patterns of difference, they do not necessarily fall out along lines that are a priori obvious. In order to determine whether there were differences in system level hub formation between our specialized and non-specialized networks, we compared the mean reduction in Theil indices for each knock-out simulation across the two groups, in Table 8. In order to determine whether these differences were significant between groups, we performed a two-sample $t$ -test. We find that none of the changes in the Theil indices between specialized and non-specialized networks within our simulation results were significantly different, suggesting that each mechanism has the same impact, on average, in both categories.

Table 8. Mean percentage change in Theil index by group, under mechanism knock-out. For removed terms, PA = preferential attachment, PS = p-shifts, ICR = ICR covariate, all=all hub-forming mechanisms

We also compare the differences in the mean reduction in Theil indices for each knock-out simulation between differently sized networks, in Table 8. To test whether these size differences were significantly different we conducted a Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance for three differently sized groups. As with the differences among specialist and non-specialist categories, we find no significant differences between the size of the networks and the mean reduction in Theil indices.

6. Discussion

In this study we focused on understanding the process of emergent coordination within the context of an unfolding disaster. We did so by investigating networks from the 2001 WTC disaster, providing empirical evidence for the role of conversational norms, preferential attachment, and institutional status in shaping both the local and system level structure and dynamics. From prior studies, it was known that the WTC radio networks had hub-like structures that emerged during the communication process. It was also known that while an individual in an ICR would have a greater likelihood of inhabiting a hub position, it was more often the case that these hubs were emergent, meaning they were occupied by individuals without institutional coordinator status. Our analysis takes this prior work one step further by modeling and simulating these networks, allowing us to understand which factors tend to guide communication, and how these same factors affect the overall structure. We did this by determining which mechanisms from the space of plausible effects were active in each network, and by estimating the strength and direction of effects that were present. We then conducted a knock-out simulation experiment to understand how the three classes of potential hub formation mechanisms impact emergent structure, comparing network Theil indices of communication volume with and without each of the respective effects. Lastly, we compared our results from both analyses across various groups to search for systematic differences by specialization and size. Overall, we find that despite the differing contexts, environments, roles, specializations, and even sizes, the results across all networks are far more similar than different. When mechanisms appear, they generally have the same sign (and usually are of comparable magnitude). Deviations from these central tendencies seem idiosyncratic and do not suggest a clear pattern of differences by size or specialization. In terms of local effects on conversation, there is a dominance of p-shift effects being present in all networks, while ICR and preferential attachment only appear in particular cases; this showcases the significance of radio SOP and conversational norms. Again, it is striking that regardless of whether individuals are communicating at ground zero, or responding to an event happening in the next state over, the communication patterns we observe are surprisingly robust.

In terms of the simulation knock-out experiment results, the first general observation is that almost all of the findings are significant, meaning that when you systematically take out any of the hypothesized hub-forming mechanisms, networks on average show large reductions in the Theil index of communication volume from their baseline state. Further, we find that the largest influencing factor is conversational inertia, which represents a 76% decrease on average in the Theil index of communication volume when the effect is knocked out. This was followed by the ICR effect and the preferential attachment effect, with reductions of 40%, and 32%, respectively. In particular, these findings suggest that conversational norms are strong drivers of emergent coordination (as suggested by Gibson et al., Reference Gibson, Buchler, Hoffman and La Fleur2019), even during an unfolding disaster—findings that also accord with participation shift usage of pilots during the unfolding Air France 447 incident (David & Schraagen, Reference David and Schraagen2018). While both ICR and preferential attachment do seem to affect the formation of coordination hubs, they do not seem to be the dominant force. The low impact from our ICR term tells us that individuals are likely to form hubs and coordinate organically in situations of disaster and that ICRs are not a necessary requirement for emergency coordination to take place. Preferential attachment’s lower level of impact on our index tells us that while preferential attachment has an important role to play in disaster communication, it is not the sole driver in every context. Our results lead us to believe that more consideration should be given to alternative forms of coordination in future literature.

By comparing REM coefficients as well as simulated behavior, we show that coordination arises in these communication networks through surprisingly similar means; what we are seeing is an emergent macro sociological phenomenon of human communication during a hazard event. It seems that by and large the most important factor driving the emergent coordination is conversational inertia. In particular, the AB-BA call and response participation shift was found in every single network after model optimization, while the AB-XA source attraction, and AB-BY “handing off” terms were present in all but one of the networks—compare that to the preferential attachment term appearing in 14 networks, and the ICR term appearing in only 11. Because of the robustness of our findings across various categorizations, we believe these results would likely reappear in other hazard or disaster contexts. Further, we believe that this might even transfer in part to non-disaster contexts that are particularly influenced by conversational norms. Researchers going back to the late 1960s and 1970s have documented the importance of conversational norms in governing social interactions (Schegloff, Reference Schegloff1968; Schank, Reference Schank1977); these results show that these humble aspects of micro-interaction have important macroscopic consequences, and that those hold even in the midst of an unfolding disaster.

7. Future studies

An obvious question for future work is whether the patterns seen here generalize to other hazard communication contexts. While these results appear to be quite robust—with regular patterns across networks of different size, composition, and task orientation—all networks reflect groups responding at the same time to the same event using the same technology, and some aspects may differ in other settings. For instance, in our study’s context, individuals were restricted to a single channel of voice communication, greatly amplifying the importance of conversational norms for communication. It is possible that other media with less dependence upon such norms might yield a correspondingly reduced impact of conversational norms on hub formation, or simply less hub formation overall. In this vein, it would be natural to probe the emergence of coordination in other single channel communication media, like singular chat rooms (NWSChat), and in multi-channel communication media, like slack or discord. Do these newer forms of communication media create new kinds of norms, or hinder certain kinds of interactions?

While our study focuses on the communication dynamics of intra-organizational members, it is an open question whether similar properties hold in settings with a mixture of inter-organizational communicators. A challenge for this line of inquiry is the need to consider larger, heterogeneous systems, which will be much harder to model and will require more extensive information on actors and the physical/institutional environment in which they are embedded (a challenge not unique to REMs). In a different vein, our approach could also provide a basis for further studies like the ongoing debate on organizational decentralization vs centralization in the context of hazards and disasters, as features like ICR or pre-training efforts on radio SOP can be directly probed (Weick, Reference Weick1993; Leonard & Howitt, Reference Leonard and Howitt2010). Although our study is rooted in a hazard context, other organizational settings involving rapid response to high-consequence events may pose related challenges; it is thus natural to speculate that some of the effects seen here may also arise in these other situations. Protocols like those used here could be used to probe such questions in a wide range of settings.

While we have considered a number of theoretically plausible mechanisms, one can posit both alternative contributors to hub formation and additional structural targets for investigation. Further studies of the WTC communication data may thus continue to yield insights, particularly as our understanding of REMs continues to develop. For instance, temporally local analogs of the triadic closure or attachment mechanisms investigated here (with temporal decay effects) may probe differential attention to recent versus older events, and latent variable models (like those of DuBois et al., Reference DuBois, Butts and Smyth2013b) may prove useful in improving performance on large and geographically extended networks. As our repertoire evolves, these new tools will provide further leverage in tackling complex social dynamics, like those seen in the WTC response.

Our results also suggest fruitful directions for inquiry into the microprocesses that drive social structure. In our study, we found that preferential attachment—despite being widely argued to be a core mechanism for hub formation in social networks—was not the dominant contributing factor in the WTC communication networks. This leads us to believe that in future studies, where hub formation is of interest, more careful theoretical consideration should be paid to alternative mechanisms. In this case, conversational inertia (primarily reciprocal turn-taking) played the largest role in coordination across multiple communication contexts, a novel finding for disaster communication networks. It is plausible that concentration in other networks may also stem from similar microdynamics. Since such behavior is “invisible” in conventional network studies, caution is needed when interpreting macroscopic patterns; formal development that links unobserved micro-processes with network structure (e.g., Butts, Reference Butts2020 in an ERGM setting, or Snijders, Reference Snijders2001 for panel dynamics) is helpful in this regard, but gaining data on microdynamics when possible is certainly advisable.

Finally, researchers may want to more broadly consider the implications of “how” an organization communicates, and what norms are set by and exist in certain media channels, as they could greatly shape the potential for organizations to deal with ongoing and future threats. This may involve taking an inventory of the various communication media an organization uses officially or unofficially, assessing how robust they are to infrastructure failure, and assessing whether secondary forms of communication may serve as primary forms in an active hazard or disaster event. Lastly, it should be considered how the alternative or secondary communication forms might hinder the effectiveness of their potential communication process. The observation that reciprocity can, when combined with other common mechanisms, drive high levels of centralization may also suggest attention to communication norms or protocols. In settings for which centralization is undesirable, inhibiting reciprocation may be a useful step. Such interventions should, however, be made with a view towards the functional importance of reciprocal turn-taking, and should weigh the potential benefits of decentralization against the potential to disrupt otherwise effective communication patterns.

8. Conclusion

Even at this temporal remove, the WTC case remains unique in the lens it provides on the detailed dynamics of the emergency phase of a disaster, and it continues to offer lessons in the drivers of coordinative behavior. In this special issue recognizing progress in REMs since their introduction, it seems apposite to provide a complete analysis of the data that motivated the initial work. The accompanying data release will likewise be a useful resource for others who wish to investigate this important historical case. Our findings point to an emergent macro sociological phenomenon, finding that even in chaotic disaster events, and diverse contexts, actors in these networks by and large utilized the conversational norms we use in everyday life.

Competing interests

None.

A. Appendix

A.1 Supplemental P-Shift simulation

While radio SOP arguably imposes multiple p-shifts as a single “package” of conversational norms, our knock-out procedure can also be used to examine their individual impacts on hub formation. Here, we provide a supplemental analysis following the same procedure as the original knock-out study, in which each p-shift term present in each original model was selectively removed. This allows us to assess the extent to which this element of the conversational “package” enhanced or inhibited hub formation.

Tables A1 and A2 show the mean Theil indices and relative changes in Theil indices for disabling each p-shift term in each network. The results show that AB-BA participation shifts play the largest role in driving the hub formation process, with a 65% reduction in the mean Theil index upon removal. This effect is followed by AB-XB and AB-BY with a reduction in the mean Theil index of 6.3% and 5.4%, respectively. Interestingly, we do not see systematic patterns in the roles played by non-AB-BA shifts, with such shifts having positive, negative, or (most often) null effects on hub formation. Thus, it appears that simple turn-taking reciprocity plays a significant part in creating centralized networks, keeping communication concentrated within a small number of individuals.

Table A1. Mean Theil index before and after p-shift knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms

Table A2. Percentage change in Theil index of communication volume p-shift knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms. $p$ -values reflect two-sample $t$ -tests (knock-out vs. full model)

$^{*}$ $p\lt 0.05$ ; $^{**}$ $p\lt 0.01$ ; $^{***}$ $p\lt 0.001$

Footnotes

Guest Editors (Special Issue on Relational Event Models): Alessandro Lomi, Tom Snijders, Christoph Stadtfeld

1 While hubs are not the only structure that could be of interest here, they are highlighted in previous findings as being prominent, ubiquitous across all WTC networks, and not largely emergent, and are therefore the focus of this paper.

2 While not normally thought of as a driver of hub formation, it is apparent that such micro-level processes could have this effect, and given the central importance of radio SOP to effective communication in events, such as the WTC disaster, we investigate its potential impact.

3 Contrast with a process in which a mechanism is removed, and the model is then re-calibrated prior to simulation. This last is more analogous to a “gain of function” (or “restoration of function”) experiment, in which we first disable some essential component of a system, and then look to see if we can recover the lost function by tweaking the system that remains.

References

Abe, K. (1976). The behavior of survivors and victims in a Japanese nightclub fire: A descriptive research note. Mass Emergencies, 1, 119124.Google Scholar
Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716723.Google Scholar
Auf der Heide, E. (1989). Disaster response: Principles of preparation and coordination. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.Google Scholar
Auf der Heide, E. (2004). Common misconceptions about disasters: Panic, the “disaster syndrome,” and looting. In O’Leary, M. (Ed.), The first 72 hours: A community approach to disaster preparedness (pp. 340380). Lincoln, Nebraska: iUniverse, Chap. 27.Google Scholar
Bavelas, A., & Barrett, D. (1951). An experimental approach to organizational communication. Personnel, 27, 366371.Google Scholar
Borrell, L. N., & Talih, M. (2011). A symmetrized theil index measure of health disparities: An example using dental caries in U.S. children and adolescents. Statistics in Medicine, 30(3), 277290.Google Scholar
Bozdogan, H. (2000). Akaike’s information criterion and recent developments in information complexity. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44(1), 6191.Google Scholar
Butts, C. T. (2008). A relational event framework for social action. Sociological Methodology, 38(1), 155200.Google Scholar
Butts, C. T. (2013). relevent: Relational event models. R package version 1.2-1, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=relevent.Google Scholar
Butts, C. T. (2020). A dynamic process reference model for sparse networks with reciprocity. Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 46(1), 1–27.Google Scholar
Butts, C. T., Petrescu-Prahova, M., & Cross, B. R. (2007). Responder communication networks in the world trade center disaster: Implications for modeling of communication within emergency settings. Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 31(2), 121147.Google Scholar
David, L. Z., & Schraagen, J. M. (2018). Analysing communication dynamics at the transaction level: The case of Air France flight 447. Cognition, Technology and Work, 20(4), 637649.Google Scholar
Drabek, T. E. (1986). Human system responses to disaster: An inventory of sociological findings. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
DuBois, C., Butts, C. T., McFarland, D., & Smyth, P. (2013a). Hierarchical models for relational event sequences. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 57(6), 297309.Google Scholar
DuBois, C., Butts, C. T., & Smyth, P. (2013b). Stochastic blockmodeling of relational event dynamics. Proceedings of the 16th international conference on artificial intelligence and statistics (aistats). Google Scholar
Dynes, R. R. (1970). Organized behavior in disaster. Lexington, MA: Heath Lexington.Google Scholar
Fararo, T. J., & Sunshine, M. H. (1964). A study of a biased friendship net. Syracuse, NY: Youth Development Center.Google Scholar
FEMA (1984). Federal Emergency Management Agency: Objectives for Local Emergency Management, CPG 1-5. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Fischer, H. W. I. I. I. (1998). Response to disaster: Fact versus fiction and its perpetuation: The sociology of disaster. New York: University Press of America.Google Scholar
Fisek, H. H., Berger, J., & Norman, R. Z. (1991). Participation in heterogeneous and homogeneous groups: A theoretical investigation. American Journal of Sociology, 97(1), 114142.Google Scholar
Fitzhugh, S. M., & Butts, C. T. (2021). Staying connected under fire: Effects of individual roles and organizational specialization on the robustness of emergency-phase communication networks. Social Networks, 64, 115.Google Scholar
Gibson, D. R. (2003). Participation shifts: Order and differentiation in group conversation. Social Forces, 81(4), 13351381.Google Scholar
Gibson, C. B., Buchler, N., Hoffman, B., & La Fleur, C.-G. (2019). Participation shifts explain degree distributions in a human communications network. PLoS ONE, 14(5), e0217240.Google Scholar
Gibson, B., Hoffman, B., Fleur, C.-G. L., & Buchler, N. (2021). Endogeneity and permeation in an organizational communication network. Social Networks, 66, 6571.Google Scholar
Kang, S. (2016). Inequality and crime revisited: Effects of local inequality and economic segregation on crime. Journal of Population Economics, 29(2), 593626.Google Scholar
Laird, J. G., Gardner, S. H., Kopel, A. J., Kerov, V., Lee, A., & Baker, S. A. (2019). Rescue of rod synapses by induction of cav alpha 1f in the mature cav1.4 knock-out mouse retina. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 60(3), 31503161.Google Scholar
Leonard, H. B., & Howitt, A. M. (2010). Organising response to extreme emergencies: The victorian bushfires of 2009. Australian Journal of Public Administration, 69(4), 372386.Google Scholar
Manz, K. M., & Mansmann, U. (2021). Inequality indices to monitor geographic differences in incidence, mortality and fatality rates over time during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS One, 16(5), e0251366.Google Scholar
McElroy, G. (2005). A short history of the handheld transceiver. QST, 89(1), 4750.Google Scholar
Mendonça, D., Webb, G., Butts, C. T., & Brooks, J. (2014). Cognitive correlates of improvised behaviour in disaster response: The cases of the Murrah Building and the world trade center. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 22(4), 185195.Google Scholar
Merton, R. K. (1968). The matthew effect in science: The reward and communication systems of science are considered. Science, 159(3810), 5663.Google Scholar
Mileti, D. S., Drabek, T. E., & Haas, J. E. (1975). Human Systems in Extreme Environments. Boulder, CO: Institute of Behavioral Science, University of Colorado.Google Scholar
Noji, E. K. (1997). Earthquakes. In: Noji, E. K. (Ed.), The Public Health Consequences of Disasters (pp. 135178). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Petrescu-Prahova, M., & Butts, C. T. (2008). Emergent coordinators in the world trade center disaster. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 28(3), 133168.Google Scholar
Price, D. J. S. (1976). A general theory of bibliometric and other cumulative advantage processes. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 27, 292306.Google Scholar
Quarantelli, E. L. (1960). Images of withdrawal behavior in disasters: Some basic misconceptions. Social Problems, 8(1), 6879.Google Scholar
Quarantelli, E. L. (1966). Organizations under stress. In: Brictson, R. (Ed.), Symposium on emergency operations (pp. 319). Santa Monica, CA: Systems Development Corporation.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rørth, P., Szabo, K., Bailey, A., Laverty, T., Rehm, J., Rubin, G. M., … Cohen, S. M. (1998). Systematic gain-of-function genetics in drosophila. Developement, 125(6), 10491057.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. (1977). Rules and topics in conversation. Cognitive Science, 1(4), 421441.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1968). Sequencing in conversational openings. American Anthropologist, 70(6), 10751095.Google Scholar
Serra, R., Villani, M., & Semeria, A. (2004). Genetic network models and statistical properties of gene expression data in knock-out experiments. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 227(1), 149157.Google Scholar
Silva, J., & Leichenko, R. (2009). Regional income ineqaulity and international trade. Economic Geography, 80(3), 261–286.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1955). On a class of skew distribution functions. Biometrika, 42(3-4), 425440.Google Scholar
Snijders, T. A. B. (2001). The statistical evaluation of social network dynamics. Sociological Methodology, 31(1), 361395.Google Scholar
Stallings, R. A. (1978). The structural patterns of four types of organizations in disaster. In Quarentelli, E. L. (Ed.), Disasters: Theory and Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Thinking about answers: The application of cognitive processes to survey methodology. San: Francisco, Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Theil, H. (1967). Economics and information theory. In Studies in Mathematical and Mangerial Economics, Vol. 7. North-Holland.Google Scholar
Tierney, K. J. (1985). Emergency medical preparedness and response in disasters: The need for interorganizational coordination. Public Administrative Review, 45, 77.Google Scholar
Tierney, K. J., Bevc, C., & Kuligowski, E. (2006). Metaphors matter: Disaster myths, media frames, and their consequences in Hurricane Katrina. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 604(1), 5781.Google Scholar
Vu, D., Lomi, A., Mascia, D., & Pallotti, F. (2017). Relational event models for longitudinal network data with an application to interhospital patient transfers. Statistics in Medicine, 36(14), 22652287.Google Scholar
Wachtendorf, T. (2004). Improvising 9/11: Organizational improvisation following the world trade center disaster. Ph.D. thesis, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Weick, K. E. (1993). The collapse of sensemaking in organizations: The mann gulch disaster. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38(4), 628.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Visualization of the time-marginalized WTC networks, sorted by specialization. All networks show high levels of centralization, with a relatively small number of coordinators providing much of the connectivity in each case.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary statistics for the WTC radio networks

Figure 2

Table 2. Model adequacy checks. Observed and null probabilities of matching features of the next event in each sequence (“Eiher” implies that sender or receiver match, while “All” implies that both sender and receiver match). All models correctly identify the next event with probability greatly exceeding the null model, on average doing so the majority of the time. Recall columns show the fraction of observed events covered by the respective fraction of probability-ordered predictions (higher is better)

Figure 3

Figure 2. The 95% simulation intervals of the Theil index of communication volume for all AICc-selected models, by network. Grey boxes display the inner quartile range, while the whiskers span the 0.025 to 97.5 percentiles. Mean value is shown in red and observed Theil index value is shown in blue.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Posterior modes and their asymptotic 95% posterior intervals for the event model parameters for all 17 WTC radio communication networks. Darker colored line segments represent specialist networks, while lightly colored line segments represent non-specialist networks. Term codes are as follows: PA, preferential attachment; P, persistence; Rr, recency of receipt; Rs, recency of sending; ICR, role effect; T-OTP, outgoing two-path; I-ITP, incoming two-path; T-ISP, incoming shared partners; T-OSP, outgoing shared partners; PS-$^{*}$, p-shift effects.

Figure 5

Table 3. Posterior means and standard deviations for AICc-selected models for the specialist networks

Figure 6

Table 4. Posterior means and standard deviations for AICc-selected models for the non-specialist networks

Figure 7

Table 5. Direction for included effects, selected models. Positive coefficients are shown in green, negative in orange; effects not included are blank. Most mechanisms show consistent effects across the networks in which they are present

Figure 8

Figure 4. Excess concentration in communication volume (above no-hub mechanism baseline) as a function of knock-out condition; values of 1.0 correspond to full model. Networks differ in importance of ICR and PA effects for hub formation, while removing p-shift effects nearly always greatly reduces concentration. (Note: concentration outside the 0-1 interval is possible.).

Figure 9

Table 6. Mean Theil index before and after mechanism knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms; for removed terms, PA = preferential attachment, PS = p-shifts, ICR = ICR covariate, all=all hub-forming mechanisms

Figure 10

Table 7. Percentage change in Theil index of communication volume mechanism knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms; for removed terms, PA = preferential attachment, PS = p-shifts, ICR = ICR covariate, all=all hub-forming mechanisms. $p$-values reflect two-sample $t$-tests (knock-out vs. full model)

Figure 11

Table 8. Mean percentage change in Theil index by group, under mechanism knock-out. For removed terms, PA = preferential attachment, PS = p-shifts, ICR = ICR covariate, all=all hub-forming mechanisms

Figure 12

Table A1. Mean Theil index before and after p-shift knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms

Figure 13

Table A2. Percentage change in Theil index of communication volume p-shift knock-out. Full model includes all AICc-selected terms. $p$-values reflect two-sample $t$-tests (knock-out vs. full model)