Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T11:30:14.227Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The contribution of affective content to cue-response correspondence in a word association task: Focus on emotion words and emotion-laden words

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2023

Ángel-Armando Betancourt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and CRAMC, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain
Marc Guasch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and CRAMC, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain
Pilar Ferré*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and CRAMC, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Pilar Ferré; Email: mariadelpilar.ferre@urv.cat

Abstract

This study aimed at examining the contribution of affective content to the organization of words in the lexicon. Based on existing free association norms and on a series of questionnaires we developed, we examined the characteristics of the words produced as associates to 840 Spanish cue words. Half of them were affective words and the other half were neutral (non-affective) words. Among the affective cue words, some words directly labeled an emotion (emotion words, EM) and others did not label an emotion but could elicit it (emotion-laden words, EL). The words produced as associates were also classified according to this distinction. Furthermore, we examined the relationship between the lexico-semantic and affective properties of the cue words and the associated words. The results revealed that EM, EL, and neutral associated words were elicited to a greater extent by cue words of the same type than by other types of cue words. Furthermore, the degree of correspondence between the affective properties of the cues and their associates was higher than that of lexico-semantic variables. These results have methodological implications for research on semantic memory and are of interest for applied studies focused on affective word organization in specific populations.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alonso, M. A., Fernandez, A., & Díez, E. (2015). Subjective age-of-acquisition norms for 7,039 Spanish words. Behavior Research Methods, 47(1), 268274. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0454-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altarriba, J. (2006). Cognitive approaches to the study of emotion-laden and emotion words in monolingual and bilingual memory. Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 56, 232.Google Scholar
Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2011). The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the Affective Simon Task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(3), 310328. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910379261 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altarriba, J., & Bauer, L. M. (2004). The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: A comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. The American Journal of Psychology, 117(3), 389410. https://doi.org/10.2307/4149007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(4), 578602. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200738 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Vol. 30, No. 1, pp. 25-36). Technical report C-1, the center for research in psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
Buades-Sitjar, F., Planchuelo, C., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2021). Valence, arousal and concreteness mediate word association. Psicothema, 33(4), 602609. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2020.484 Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, P., Lin, J., Chen, B., Lu, C., & Guo, T. (2015). Processing emotional words in two languages with one brain: ERP and fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. Cortex, 71, 3448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Citron, F. M. (2012). Neural correlates of written emotion word processing: a review of recent electrophysiological and hemodynamic neuroimaging studies. Brain and Language, 122(3), 211226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Citron, F. M., Weekes, B. S., & Ferstl, E. C. (2014). Arousal and emotional valence interact in written word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(10), 12571267. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.897734 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coronges, K. A., Stacy, A. W., & Valente, T. W. (2007). Structural comparison of cognitive associative networks in two populations 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37(9), 20972129. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00253.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., Perfors, A., Brysbaert, M., & Storms, G. (2019). The “Small World of Words” English word association norms for over 12,000 cue words. Behavior Research Methods, 51(3), 9871006. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1115-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D., & Storms, G. (2013). Associative strength and semantic activation in the mental lexicon: evidence from continued word associations. Germany: Cognitive Science Society. https://hdl.handle.net/2440/83798 Google Scholar
Delaney-Busch, N., Wilkie, G., & Kuperberg, G. (2016). Vivid: How valence and arousal influence word processing under different task demands. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(3), 415432. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0402-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diez, E., Alonso, M. A., Rodríguez, N., & Fernandez, A. (2018). Free-association norms for a large set of words in Spanish [Poster Presentation]. 2018, July 3-6. First Joint Congress of the SEPEX, SEPNECA and AIP experimental, Madrid, Spain. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.17703.70560 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, A., & Carreiras, M. (2013). EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 12461258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estes, Z., & Adelman, J. S. (2008). Automatic vigilance for negative words is categorical and general. Emotion, 8(4), 453457. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012887 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferré, P., Guasch, M., Moldovan, C., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2012). Affective norms for 380 Spanish words belonging to three different semantic categories. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 395403. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0165-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraga, I., Guasch, M., Haro, J., Padrón, I., & Ferré, P. (2018). EmoFinder: The meeting point for Spanish emotional words. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 8493. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-1006-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grömping, U. (2007). Relative importance for linear regression in R: the package relaimpo. Journal of Statistical Software, 17, 127. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v017.i01 Google Scholar
Guasch, M., Ferré, P., & Fraga, I. (2016). Spanish norms for affective and lexico-semantic variables for 1,400 words. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 13581369. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0684-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haro, J., Calvillo, R., Poch, C., Hinojosa, J. A., & Ferré, P. (2022). Your words went straight to my heart: the role of emotional prototypicality in the recognition of emotion-label words. Psychological Research (Advance Online Publication). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01723-6 Google Scholar
Hinojosa, J. A., Moreno, E. M., & Ferré, P. (2020). Affective neurolinguistics: towards a framework for reconciling language and emotion. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(7), 813839. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1620957 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinojosa, J. A., Martínez-García, N., Villalba-García, C., Fernández-Folgueiras, U., Sánchez-Carmona, A., Pozo, M. A., & Montoro, P. R. (2016a). Affective norms of 875 Spanish words for five discrete emotional categories and two emotional dimensions. Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 272284. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0572-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinojosa, J. A., Rincón-Pérez, I., Romero-Ferreiro, M. V., Martínez-García, N., Villalba-García, C., Montoro, P. R., & Pozo, M. A. (2016b). The Madrid Affective Database for Spanish (MADS): Ratings of dominance, familiarity, subjective age of acquisition and sensory experience. PLoS One, 11(5), e0155866. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155866 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, M. J., Kuchinke, L., Tamm, S., , M. L., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not positive words. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(4), 389397. https://doi.org/10.3758/9.4.389 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, Z., & Liu, H. (2019). The affective meaning of words is constrained by the conceptual meaning. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48, 13771390. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09663-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 785813. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196544 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazanas, S. A., & Altarriba, J. (2015). The automatic activation of emotion and emotion-laden words: Evidence from a masked and unmasked priming paradigm. The American Journal of Psychology, 128(3), 323336. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.3.0323 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klauer, K., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In Musch, J. & Klauer, K. C. (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 749). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021446 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kousta, S. T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112(3), 473481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumar, A. A. (2021). Semantic memory: A review of methods, models, and current challenges. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 4080. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01792-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuperman, V., Estes, Z., Brysbaert, M., & Warriner, A. B. (2014). Emotion and language: valence and arousal affect word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1065. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035669 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, R. J., Mercer, K. A., Balota, D. A., & Strube, M. J. (2008). Not all negative words slow down lexical decision and naming speed: Importance of word arousal. Emotion, 8(4), 445452. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.445 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618630. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212999 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludueña, G. A., Behzad, M. D., & Gros, C. (2014). Exploration in free word association networks: models and experiment. Cognitive Processing, 15(2), 195200. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-0590-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meteyard, L., Rodríguez-Cuadrado, S., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: a review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free association and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 887899. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palazova, M., Mantwill, K., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). Are effects of emotion in single words non-lexical? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 27662775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlenko, A. (2008). Emotion and emotion-laden words in the bilingual lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 147164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003283 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Sánchez, M. Á., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Guasch, M., Hinojosa, J. A., Fraga, I., Marín, J., & Ferré, P. (2021). EmoPro–Emotional prototypicality for 1286 Spanish words: Relationships with affective and psycholinguistic variables. Behavior Research Methods, 53(5), 18571875. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01519-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Comesaña, M., & Perea, M. (2005). Estudio normativo del valor afectivo de 478 palabras españolas. Psicológica, 26(2), 317326. http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=16926207 Google Scholar
Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (affective norms for English words). Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 600605. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., & Davies, R. (2019). The graded effect of valence on word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(5), 851868. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000616 Google ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S. L., Shapleske, J., & David, A. S. (2000). Direct and indirect semantic priming with neutral and emotional words in schizophrenia: Relationship to delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5(4), 271292. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800050199720 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 11611178. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145172. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 805819. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santos, A., Chaigneau, S. E., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Property generation reflects word association and situated simulation. Language and Cognition, 3(1), 83119. https://doi.org/10.1515/LANGCOG.2011.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Time course and task dependence of emotion effects in word processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(1), 2843. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.1.28 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2014). Emotion words and categories: Evidence from lexical decision. Cognitive Processing, 15(2), 209215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-0589-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Imbault, C., Pérez Sánchez, M. A., & Brysbaert, M. (2017). Norms of valence and arousal for 14,031 Spanish words. Behavior Research Methods, 49(1), 111123. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0700-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Rensbergen, B., De Deyne, S., & Storms, G. (2016). Estimating affective word covariates using word association data. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 16441652. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0680-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Rensbergen, B., Storms, G., & De Deyne, S. (2015). Examining assortativity in the mental lexicon: Evidence from word associations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 17171724. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0832-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinson, D., Ponari, M., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). How does emotional content affect lexical processing? Cognition and Emotion, 28(4), 737746. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.851068 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S., Chan, K. Y., & Goldstein, R. (2014). Insights into failed lexical retrieval from network science. Cognitive Psychology, 68, 132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.10.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vivas, L., Manoiloff, L., García, A. M., Lizarralde, F., & Vivas, J. (2019). Core semantic links or lexical associations: assessing the nature of responses in word association tasks. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48(1), 243256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9601-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, X., Shangguan, C., & Lu, J. (2019). Time course of emotion effects during emotion-label and emotion-laden word processing. Neuroscience Letters, 699, 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2019.01.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 11911207. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, T. Y. Q., Yap, M. J., Obana, T., Asplund, C., & Teh, E. (2022). Brief report: Emotional picture and language processing in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 52, 435446. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04920-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, C., & Zhang, J. (2020). Emotion word type should be incorporated in affective neurolinguistics: a commentary on Hinojosa, Moreno and Ferré (2019). Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35, 840843. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1696979 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Ch., Zhang, J., & Yuan, Z. (2021). Exploring affective priming effect of emotion-label words and emotion-laden words: An event-related potential study. Brain Sciences, 11(5), 553. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11050553 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yao, Z., Yu, D., Wang, L., Zhu, X., Guo, J., & Wang, Z. (2016). Effects of valence and arousal on emotional word processing are modulated by concreteness: Behavioral and ERP evidence from a lexical decision task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110, 231242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.07.499 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, M. J., & Seow, C. S. (2014). The influence of emotion on lexical processing: Insights from RT distributional analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 526533. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0525-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., Wu, C., Meng, Y., & Yuan, Z. (2017). Different neural correlates of emotion-label words and emotion-laden words: An ERP study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 455. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00455 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., Teo, T., & Wu, C. (2019). Emotion words modulate early conflict processing in a flanker task: differentiating emotion-label words and emotion-laden words in second language. Language and Speech, 62(4), 641651. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830918807509 CrossRefGoogle Scholar