Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-03T11:17:09.338Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Acquisition of the feature [+spread glottis] in Icelandic

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2023

Thora MÁSDÓTTIR*
Affiliation:
University of Iceland, Faculty of Medicine, Division of Speech Pathology, Iceland
Barbara May BERNHARDT
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, School of Audiology and Speech Sciences, Canada
Joseph Paul STEMBERGER
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, Department of Linguistics, Canada
Gunnar Ólafur HANSSON
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, Department of Linguistics, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Thora Másdóttir; Email: tm@hi.is

Abstract

The feature [+spread glottis] ([+s.g.]) denotes that a speech sound is produced with a wide glottal aperture with audible voiceless airflow. Icelandic is unusual in the degree to which [+spread glottis] is involved in the phonology: in /h/, pre-aspirated and post-aspirated stops, voiceless fricatives and voiceless sonorants. The ubiquitousness of the feature could potentially affect the rate and process of its acquisition. This paper investigates the development of [+s.g.] in Icelandic, both in general and in a range of contexts, in a cross-sectional study of 433 typically developing Icelandic-speaking children aged two to seven years. As a feature, [+s.g.] is acquired early in Icelandic, although specific sound classes lag behind due to other output constraints. Children reach mastery of [+s.g.] by age three except in word-initial post-aspirated stops and voiceless nasals. Findings are interpreted in light of the literature on the feature and its development.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Árnason, K. (1986). The segmental and suprasegmental aspects of preaspiration. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 9(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586500001384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Árnason, K. (2005). Íslensk tunga I: Hljóð [Icelandic language: Sounds]. Reykjavik: Almenna bókafélagiðGoogle Scholar
Árnason, K. (2011). The phonology of Icelandic and Faroese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199229314.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (2010). Generalizing over lexicons to predict consonant mastery. Laboratory Phonology, 1(2), 319343. https://doi.org/10.1515/LABPHON.2010.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beers, W. (1995). The phonology of normally developing and language-impaired children. Amsterdam: Uitgave IFOTT.Google Scholar
Bernhardt, B. H., & Stemberger, J. P. (1998). Handbook of phonological development: From the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bombien, L. (2006). Voicing alterations in Icelandic sonorants – a photoglottographic and acoustic analysis. In Arbeitsberichte des Instituts für Phonetik und digitale Sprachverarbeitung der Universität Kiel, 37, 6382.Google Scholar
Clausen, M. C., & Fox-Boyer, A. (2017). Phonological development of Danish-speaking children: A normative cross-sectional study. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 31(6), 440458. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1308014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B., Holm, A., Hua, Z., & Crosbie, S. (2003). Phonological development: A normative study of British English-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17(8), 617643. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269920031000111348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, A. V., & Dodd, B. J. (1999). Der Erwerb des phonologischen System in der Deutschen Sprache [The acquisition of phonological systems in German]. Sprache – Stimme – Gehör, 23, 183191.Google Scholar
Gíslason, I., Konráðsson, S., & Jóhannesson, B. (1986). Framburður og myndun fleirtölu hjá 200 íslenskum börnum við fjögra og sex ára aldur [Articulation and production of plurals in the speech of 200 Icelandic children at the age of four and six years]. Reykjavík: Icelandic College of Education.Google Scholar
Halle, M., & Stevens, K. N. (1971). A note on laryngeal features. MIT Research Laboratory of Electronics Quarterly Progress Report, 101, 198213.Google Scholar
Haugen, E. (1958). The phonemics of modern Icelandic. Language 34(1), 5588. https://doi.org/10.2307/411276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedlund, G., & Rose, Y. (2020). Phon 3.1 [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://phon.ca.Google Scholar
Heimisdóttir, L. Ö. (2015). The phonology of aspiration in Icelandic: A gesture-based approach (Doctoral dissertation). Cornell University.Google Scholar
Helgason, P. (1993). On coarticulation and connected speech processes in Icelandic. Reykjavík: Institute of Linguistics, University of Iceland.Google Scholar
Holm, A., Sanchez, K., Crosbie, S., Morgan, A., & Dodd, B. (2022). Is children’s speech development changing? Preliminary evidence from Australian English-speaking 3-year-olds, International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(4), 375384. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2021.1991474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoole, P., & Bombien, L. (2010). Velar and glottal activity in Icelandic. In Mooshammer, C., Fuchs, S., Hoole, P. & Zygis, M. (Eds.), Between the regular and particular in speech and language (pp. 171204). Frankfurt am Main: Peter LangGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D. (1999). Phonological acquisition. In Barrett, M. (Ed.), The development of language (pp. 7397). London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1968/1941). Child Language, aphasia and phonological universals. New York: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jessen, M., & Pétursson, M. (1998). Voiceless nasal phonemes in Icelandic. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 28(1), 4353. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002510030000623XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jessen, M., & Ringen, C. O. (2002). Laryngeal features in German. Phonology, 19(2), 189218. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4420223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jónsson, J. G. (1994). The feature [asp] in Icelandic phonology. Studia Linguistica, 48(1), 2845. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9582.1994.tb00848.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kingston, J., & Diehl, R. (1994). Phonetic knowledge. Language, 70, 419454. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1994.0023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lodge, K. (2007). Timing, segmental status and aspiration in Icelandic. Transactions of the Philological Society, 105(1), 66104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-968X.2007.00181.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Másdóttir, T. (2008). Phonological development and disorders in Icelandic-speaking children (Doctoral dissertation). Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne.Google Scholar
Másdóttir, T. [Þóra Másdóttir] (2014). Málhljóðapróf ÞM: Handbók [ÞM’s Speech Sound Test: Manual]. Reykjavík: Heyrnar- og talmeinastöð Íslands.Google Scholar
Másdóttir, T. (2018). Word-initial /r/-clusters in Icelandic-speaking children with protracted versus typical phonological development. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 32(5-6), 424445. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1359855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Másdóttir, T. [Þóra Másdóttir] (2019). Hljóðþróun íslenskra barna á aldrinum tveggja til átta ára. [Speech sound development of Icelandic children aged two to eight years.] Netla – Veftímarit um uppeldi og menntun. https://doi.org/10.24270/netla.2019.11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Másdóttir, T., & Bernhardt, B. M. (2022). Uncommon timing variation in the speech of an Icelandic-speaking child with protracted phonological development. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 36(9), 806819. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2021.2011959CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Másdóttir, T., McLeod, S., & Crowe, K. (2021). Icelandic children’s acquisition of consonants and consonant clusters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 64, 14901502. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-20-00463CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, J. (1988). Feature geometry and dependency: A review. Phonetica, 45, 84108. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261820CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, S., & Crowe, K. (2018). Children’s consonant acquisition in 27 languages: A cross-linguistic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(4), 15461571. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-17-0100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Namasivayam, A. K., Coleman, D., O’Dwyer, A., & Van Lieshout, P. (2020). Speech sound disorders in children: An articulatory phonology perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(1), 122. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02998CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolae, A., & Nevins, A. (2016). Fricative patterning in aspirating versus true voice languages. Journal of Linguistics, 52(1), 151174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226715000067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J., Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Child phonology : Volume 1 , production (pp. 931123). New York, NY: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, K. (1996). Aspects of variability in child language acquisition. In Bernhardt, B. H., Gilbert, J. & Ingram, D. (eds.), Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition (pp. 114). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Ringen, C. O. (1999). Aspiration, preaspiration, deaspiration, sonorant devoicing and spirantization in Icelandic. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 22(2), 137156. https://doi.org/10.1080/03325860050179227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Y., & Inkelas, S. (2011). The Interpretation of phonological patterns in first language acquisition. In Ewen, C. J., Hume, E., van Oostendorp, M. & Rice, K. (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Phonology (pp. 24142438). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Shriberg, L. D., & Lof, G. L. (1991). Reliability studies in broad and narrow phonetic transcription. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 5(3), 225279. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699209108986113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smit, A. B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J. J., Bernthal, J. E., & Bird, A. (1990). The Iowa Articulation Norms Project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(4), 779798. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5504.779CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SPSS Inc. (2022). SPSS for windows version 29.0. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc./IBM.Google Scholar
Stokes, S., & Surendran, D. (2005). Articulatory complexity, ambient frequency, and functional load as predictors of consonant development in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(3), 577591. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/040)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thráinsson, H. (1978). On the phonology of Icelandic preaspiration. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 1(1), 354. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586500000196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thráinsson, H. (1981). Stuðlar, höfuðstafir, hljóðkerfi [Alliteration and the phonological system]. In Kvaran, G., Ingólfsson, G. & Sigmundsson, S. (Eds.), Afmæliskveðja til Halldórs Halldórssonar 13. júlí 1981 (pp. 110123). Reykjavík: Íslenska málfræðifélagið.Google Scholar
Vaux, B. (1998). The laryngeal specifications of fricatives. Linguistic Inquiry, 29(3), 497511. https://doi.org/10.1162/002438998553833CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, C., & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development from babbling to speech: Common tendencies and individual differencesApplied Psycholinguistics, 7(1), 340. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400007165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Velleman, S., & McCune, L. (1994). How abstract is child phonology? Toward an integration of linguistic and psychological approaches. In Yavas, M. (Ed.), First and second language phonology (pp. 944). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 1

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material
Download Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 15.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 2

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material
Download Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 23.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 3

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material
Download Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 25.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 4

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material
Download Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 19.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 5

Másdóttir et al. supplementary material
Download Másdóttir et al. supplementary material 5(File)
File 19 KB