Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T04:20:45.271Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lexical processing in children with hearing impairment in oral word reading in transparent Arabic orthography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2023

Mirna Mattar*
Affiliation:
Speech Therapy Department, Faculty of Public Health II, Lebanese University, Beirut, Lebanon Laboratoire, Cognition Langage et Développement, Center of Research in Cognition & Neurosciences, ULB Neuroscience Institute, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgium
Carole El Akiki
Affiliation:
UMR 1253, Imagery and Brain (iBrain), Inserm, Université de Tours, Tours, France
Jacqueline Leybaert
Affiliation:
Laboratoire, Cognition Langage et Développement, Center of Research in Cognition & Neurosciences, ULB Neuroscience Institute, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgium
*
Corresponding author: Mirna Mattar; Email: mirna.mattar@ul.edu.lb

Abstract

Word recognition mechanisms constitute an essential contribution to reading achievement in both deaf and hearing children. Little is known about how children with hearing impairment (HI) manage to read aloud words in the vowelled Arabic transparent script which provides full vowel information. This study aimed to compare word and pseudoword reading accuracy and speed between 32 Lebanese children with HI and 32 younger hearing Lebanese children. The two groups were carefully matched for reading comprehension and oral comprehension levels. Length, word frequency, and lexicality effects were assessed to characterize the functioning of the lexical and sublexical reading procedures. Reading errors were also analyzed to document reading difficulties in transparent Arabic orthography in the sublexical route. The results show significant effects of length, word frequency, and lexicality on reading accuracy and speed in both groups. They also indicate underdeveloped sublexical and lexical routes in children with HI who read less accurately and faster than the younger hearing children. Reading errors are numerous in children with HI. The data are discussed in light of the Dual Route Cascaded model. Suggestions are made about how to improve reading processes in children with HI.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Leil, A. K., Share, D. L., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). How does speed and accuracy in reading relate to reading comprehension in Arabic? Psicológica, 35(2), 251276.Google Scholar
Abu-rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2004). Reading and spelling error analysis of native. Reading and Writing, 17(7), 651690. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-2657-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abu-Rabia, S. (2019). The role of short vowels in reading Arabic: A critical literature review. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48, 785795. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09631-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic–Hebrew–English-speaking Arab children. Reading and Writing, 16, 611634. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025838029204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asadi, I. A. (2017). Reading Arabic with the diacritics for short vowels: Vowelised but not necessarily easy to read. Writing Systems Research, 9(2), 137147. https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2017.1400493 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asadi, I. A., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). The influence of diglossia on different types of phonological abilities in Arabic. Journal of Education and Learning, 3(3), 45. https://doi.org/10.5539/jel.v3n3p45 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakhos, J-L., Sleilaty, G., Khalaf, D., Bou Serhal, R., El Boustani, K., Lattouf, E., Bakhos, E., Samaha, N., & Abou-Mrad, F. (2022). Recognition of hearing impairment disability: First comprehensive study on the deaf in Lebanon. International Journal of Otorhinolaryngology, 8(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijo.20220801.11 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-on, A., Shalhoub-Awwad, Y., & Tuma-Basila, R. (2018). Contribution of phonological and morphological information in reading Arabic: A developmental perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 12531277. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000310 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34(3), 424434. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195471 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Millogo, V., Farioli, F., & Grainger, J. (2004). A developmental investigation of word length effects in reading using a new on-line word identification paradigm. Reading and Writing, 17(4), 411431. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:READ.0000032664.20755.af CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, A., & Howard, D. (1992). Frozen phonology thawed: The analysis and remediation of a developmental disorder of real word phonology. European Journal of Disorder of Communication, 27(4), 343365. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682829209012045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G. (2002). How early does morpholexical reading develop in readers of a shallow orthography? Brain and Language, 81(1-3), 568586. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2548CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burani, C., Thornton, A. M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2017). Learning to read Italian. In Verhoeven, L. & Perfetti, C. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 211242). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316155752.009 Google Scholar
Carver, R. P. (1990). Reading rate: A review of research and theory. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. The Science of Reading: A Handbook, 6, 2341. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch1 Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589608. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.589 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.204 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conrad, R. (1979). The deaf school child: Language and cognitive function. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Cossu, G., Gugliotta, M., & Marshall, J. C. (1995). Acquisition of reading and written spelling in a transparent orthography: Two non parallel processes? Reading and Writing, 7(1), 922. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01026945 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, P. T., & Share, D. L. (2018). Writing system variation and its consequences for reading and dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 101116. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1379082 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, R., Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Suárez, P., & Cuetos, F. (2013). Lexical and sub-lexical effects on accuracy, reaction time and response duration: Impaired and typical word and pseudoword reading in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 26(5), 721738. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9388-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominguez, A.-B., & Alegria, J. (2010). Reading mechanisms in orally educated deaf adults. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 15(2), 136148. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enp033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domínguez, A.-B., Carrillo, M.-S., González, V., & Alegria, J. (2016). How do deaf children with and without cochlear implants manage to read sentences: The key word strategy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 21(3), 280292. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enw026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowdall, N., Melendez-Torres, G. J., Murray, L., Gardner, F., Hartford, L., & Cooper, P. J. (2020). Shared picture book reading interventions for child language development: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Development, 91(2), 383399. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13225 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L., Thériault-Whalen, M., & Dunn, L. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody (EVIP). Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary test-revised. Toronto: PSYCAN.Google Scholar
El Akiki, C., & Content, A. (2020). Early sensitivity to morphology in beginning readers of Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 552315. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.552315 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eviatar, Z., Ibrahim, R., Karelitz, T. M., & Simon, A. B. (2019). Speed of reading texts in Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing, 32(3), 537559. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9877-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagan, M. K., Pisoni, D. B., Horn, D. L., & Dillon, C. M. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates of vocabulary, reading, and working memory in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(4), 461471. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, G. A. (1959). Statistical analysis in psychology and education. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E. (2003). Predictors of reading skill development in children with early cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24(1), 5968. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.AUD.0000051690.43989.5D CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1998). Word frequency effects in oral reading are not merely age-of-acquisition effects in disguise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 267283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.267 Google Scholar
Geva, E., & Siegel, L. S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing, 12, 130. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008017710115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U., Wang, H. L., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Huss, N. (2010). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 325337. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21453 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 610. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193258600700104 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, M, Terlektsi, E., & Kyle, F. E. (2017). Literacy outcomes for primary school children who are deaf and hard of hearing: A cohort comparison study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(3), 701711. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-H-15-0403 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, J. A. (1993). Reading comprehension subgroup results. American Annals of the Deaf, 138(2), 172175. https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.2012.0684 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Bowyer-Crane, C., Carroll, J. M., Duff, F. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2012). The causal role of phoneme awareness and letter–sound knowledge in learning to read: Combining intervention studies with mediation analyses. Psychological Science, 23(6), 572577. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611435921 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ibrahim, R. (2013). Reading in Arabic: New evidence for the role of vowel signs. Creative Education, 04, 248253. https://doi.org/10.4236/ce.2013.44036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacquier-Roux, M., Lequette, C., Pouget, G., Valdois, S., & Zorman, M. (2010). BALE: Batterie analytique du langage écrit. Groupe Cogni-Sciences, Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neurocognition.Google Scholar
JASP Team (2022). JASP (Version 0.16.1) [Computer Software].Google Scholar
Johnson, C., & Goswami, U. (2010). Phonological awareness, vocabulary, and reading in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 53(2), 237. https://doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0139) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karchmer, M. A., & Mitchell, R. E. (2003). Demographic and achievement characteristics of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education, 38, 2137.Google Scholar
Kassir, H. (2015). Elaboration d’un test évaluant la compréhension morphosyntaxique orale chez l’enfant libanais âgé de 4 à 6 ans [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Lebanese University.Google Scholar
Khateb, A., Khateb-Abdelgani, M., Taha, H.Y., & Ibrahim, R. (2014). The impact of orthographic connectivity on visual word recognition in Arabic: A cross-sectional study. Reading and Writing, 27, 14131436. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9499-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoors, H., Brons, M., & Marschark, M. (2019). Deaf education beyond the western world: Context, challenges, and prospect. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2014). Reading speed and phonological awareness deficits among Arabic-speaking children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 21(1), 8095. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1491 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leybaert, J. (1993). Reading in deaf children. In Marschark, M. & Clark, D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness. Owford University Press.Google Scholar
Leybaert, J., & Content, A. (1995). Reading and spelling acquisition in two teaching methods: a test of the independence hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 6588. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01026948 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lobrot, M., & Adnet-Piat, E. (1973). Lire: Avec épreuves pour évaluer la capacité de lecture (D-OR-LEC). Sciences de l’éducation.Google Scholar
Lowell, R., & Morris, R. K. (2014). Word length effects on novel words: Evidence from eye movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(1), 179189. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0556-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maamouri, M. (1998). Language education and human development: Arabic diglossia and its impact on the quality of education in the Arab region.Google Scholar
Marinelli, C. V., Vizzi, F., & Zoccolotti, P. (2019). Reading skills in deaf subjects: Role of psycholinguistic factors and global influences in affecting reading performance. Psychology, 10, 17541775. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2019.1012115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maroun, L., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2019). Visual and orthographic processing in Arabic word recognition among dyslexic and typical readers. Writing Systems Research, 11(2), 142158. https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2020.1764159 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., del Giudice, A. A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2011). Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), 164188. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enq049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, C., Trezek, B. J., & Hancock, G. R. (2021). Reading achievement of deaf students: Challenging the fourth grade ceiling. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 26(3), 427437. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enab013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno-Pérez, F. J., Saldaña, D., & Rodríguez-Ortiz, I. R. (2015). Reading efficiency of deaf and hearing people in Spanish. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 20(4), 374384. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/env030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In Gough, P.B., Ehri, L.C. & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145174). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 3250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pimperton, H., Blythe, H., Kreppner, J., Mahon, M., Peacock, J. L., Stevenson, J., Terlektsi, E., Worsfold, S., Yuen, H. M., & Kennedy, C. R. (2016). The impact of universal newborn hearing screening on long-term literacy outcomes: A prospective cohort study. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 101(1), 915. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-307516 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qi, S., & Mitchell, R. E. (2012). Large-scale academic achievement testing of deaf and hard-of-hearing students: Past, present, and future. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 17(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enr028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Recommandation BIAP 02/1. (1997). Structures en audiophonologie. https://www.biap.org/.Google Scholar
Rees, R. I. (2009). Deaf children’s acquisition of speech skills: A psycholinguistic perspective through intervention [Doctoral dissertation], University College London. https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/1348395 Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing, 18(6), 559582. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3180-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2012). Literacy reflexes of Arabic diglossia. In Leikin, M., Schwartz, M., & Tobin, Y. (Eds.), Current issues in bilingualism (pp. 4355). Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2327-6_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2017). MAWRID: A model of Arabic word reading in development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(5), 454462. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219417720460 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Henkin-Roitfarb, R. (2014). The structure of Arabic language and orthography. Handbook of Arabic Literacy, 9, 328. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-8545-7_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Schiff, R. (2016). The impact of diglossia on vowelled and unvowelled word reading in Arabic: A developmental study from childhood to adolescence. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(4), 311324. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2016.1180526 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, R., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2018). Development and relationships between phonological awareness, morphological awareness and word reading in spoken and standard Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 356. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shany, M., Bar-On, A., & Katzir, T. (2012). Reading different orthographic structures in the shallow-pointed Hebrew script: A cross-grade study in elementary school. Reading and Writing, 25(6), 12171238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9314-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95129. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1998.2481 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D.L., & Bar-On, A. (2018). Learning to read a Semitic Abjad: The triplex model of Hebrew reading development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(5), 444453. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219417718198 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (Eds.). (2005). The science of reading: A handbook. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Spencer, L. J., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Evaluating phonological processing skills in children with prelingual deafness who use cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14, 221. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enn013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spencer, P. E., & Marschark, M. (2010). Evidence-based practice in educating deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 217235. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2702_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., & Bonnet, P. (1998). Reading and spelling acquisition in French: The role of phonological, mediation and orthographic factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68(2), 134165. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1997.2422 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanwick, R., & Watson, L. (2007). Parents sharing books with young deaf children in spoken English and in BSL: The common and diverse features of different language settings. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(3), 385405. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taha, H. (2016). Deep and shallow in Arabic orthography: New evidence from reading performance of elementary school native Arab readers. Writing Systems Research, 8(2), 133142. https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2015.1114910 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R. (2018). Investigating phonological awareness and naming speed as predictors of reading in Arabic. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 7084. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1340948 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trezek, B. J., Wang, Y., Woods, D. G., Gampp, T. L., & Paul, P. V. (2007). Using visual phonics to supplement beginning reading instruction for students who are deaf or hard of Hearing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(3), 373384. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdois, S., Carbonnel, S., Juphard, A., Baciu, M., Ans, B., Peyrin, C., & Segebarth, C. (2006). Polysyllabic pseudo-word processing in reading and lexical decision: Converging evidence from behavioral data, connectionist simulations and functional MRI. Brain Research, 1085, 149162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.02.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdois, S., Reilhac, C., Ginestet, E., & Line Bosse, M. (2021). Varieties of cognitive profiles in poor readers: Evidence for a VAS-impaired subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 54(3), 221233. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219420961332 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wass, M., Ching, T. Y., Cupples, L., Wang, H. C., Lyxell, B., Martin, L., Button, L., Gunnourie, M., Boisvert, I., McMahon, C., & Castles, A. (2019). Orthographic learning in children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 333350(1), 99112. https://doi.org/10.1044/20181_LSHSS-17-0146 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yap, M. J., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(4), 502529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 93(3), 369373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed