Skip to main content
Log in

Proactive response preparation contributes to contingency learning: novel evidence from force-sensitive keyboards

  • Research
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Contingency learning can involve learning that the identity of one stimulus in a sequence predicts the identity of the next stimulus. It remains unclear, however, whether such learning speeds responses to the next stimulus only by reducing the threshold for triggering the expected response after stimulus onset or also by preparing the expected response before stimulus onset. To distinguish between these competing accounts, we manipulated the probabilities with which each of two prime arrows (Left and Right) were followed by each of two probe arrows (Up and Down) in a prime-probe task while using force-sensitive keyboards to monitor sub-threshold finger force. Consistent with the response preparation account, two experiments revealed greater force just before probe onset on the response key corresponding to the direction in which the probe was more (versus less) likely to point (e.g., Up vs. Down). Furthermore, mirroring sequential contingency effects in behavior, this pre-probe force effect vanished after a single low-probability trial. These findings favor the response preparation account over the threshold only account. They also suggest the possibility that contingency learning in our tasks indexes trial-by-trial expectations regarding the utility of the prime for predicting the upcoming probe.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The preregistration, task scripts, data analysis scripts, and raw data are available on the Open Science Framework (OSF): Experiment 1 (https://osf.io/jd9ys/) and Experiment 2 (https://osf.io/eyzux/).

Notes

  1. In these studies, contingency learning effects appear when the prime and probe appear simultaneously. Such effects, however, are stronger when the prime appears before the probe (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2016b).

  2. The finding may alternatively be explained by the fact that the relative frequencies with which high- and medium-contingency trials appeared (50% vs. 25%) differed more than the relative frequencies with which medium- and low-frequency trials appeared (25% vs. 17%).

  3. We mistakenly reported this partial-eta-squared value as 0.41 in our pre-registration.

  4. To be consistent with our prior studies of the prime-probe task, we used the same key-hand mapping for all participants, rather than mapping the left and right arrow-direction keys to the left hand and the up and down arrow-direction keys to the right hand in half the participants and using the opposite key-hand mapping in the other half. Using a constant mapping does not lead to a design confound because we use the same mapping in all conditions.

  5. We identified outliers using both correct and error RTs in the analysis of the mean ER data, not only correct RTs as in the analysis of the mean RT data. Thus, these analyses produced slightly different percentages of outliers.

  6. In our pre-registration, we stated that we would analyze force in trials with correct responses to both the prime and the probe. An anonymous reviewer pointed out a valid concern with this approach, however. Excluding trials with an incorrect response to the probe could bias the results to show greater pre-probe force on the high-contingency response key than on the low-contingency response key. Indeed, randomly increasing pre-probe force on the high-contingency response key (e.g., due to guessing) could facilitate a correct probe response such that the trial is included in the analysis. In contrast, randomly increasing pre-probe force on the low-contingency response key could facilitate an incorrect probe response such that the trial is not included. To prevent such a bias, we analyzed pre-probe force regardless of whether the subsequent probe response was correct. We note that this change to our pre-registered analysis plan did not change any of the inferences that we describe below.

  7. The authors thank Alexander Weigard at the University of Michigan for bringing this possibility to their attention.

References

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Princess Ativie, Matthew Dunaway, Sophia Hopkins, Katherine Ni, Carlin Pendell, Chloe Saba, and Nancy Villagomez for assistance with data collection.

Funding

This work was funded by discretionary funds to Daniel H. Weissman from the University of Michigan.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DHW conceptualized the study. Both authors contributed to the design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by DHW. DHW wrote the first draft of the manuscript and both authors commented on all versions of the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel H. Weissman.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing financial or non-financial interests that are directly or indirectly related to the work submitted for publication.

Ethical approval

The University of Michigan’s Behavioral Sciences Internal Review Board deemed that the present study was exempt from oversight because they only involved simple cognitive tasks. The experiments were performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helskini and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Weissman, D.H., Schmidt, J.R. Proactive response preparation contributes to contingency learning: novel evidence from force-sensitive keyboards. Psychological Research (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-01940-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-01940-1

Navigation