Next Article in Journal
Sense of Agency and Skills Learning in Virtual-Mediated Environment: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
Use of Prescribed and Non-Prescribed Treatments for Cluster Headache in a Swedish Cohort
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

Investigating Dyslexia through Diffusion Tensor Imaging across Ages: A Systematic Review

by
Bruce Martins
1,
Mariana Yumi Baba
1,
Elisa Monteiro Dimateo
1,
Leticia Fruchi Costa
2,
Aila Silveira Camara
2,
Katerina Lukasova
2 and
Mariana Penteado Nucci
1,*
1
Laboratório de Investigação Médica em Neurorradiologia—LIM44—Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo 05403-000, Brazil
2
Centro de Matemática, Computação e Cognição (CMCC), Universidade Federal do ABC, Santo André 09210-580, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(4), 349; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14040349
Submission received: 29 February 2024 / Revised: 17 March 2024 / Accepted: 28 March 2024 / Published: 31 March 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Research on Bio-Behavioral Signatures of Neurodevelopmental Disorders)

Abstract

:
Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that presents a deficit in accuracy and/or fluency while reading or spelling that is not expected given the level of cognitive functioning. Research indicates brain structural changes mainly in the left hemisphere, comprising arcuate fasciculus (AF) and corona radiata (CR). The purpose of this systematic review is to better understand the possible methods for analyzing Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) data while accounting for the characteristics of dyslexia in the last decade of the literature. Among 124 articles screened from PubMed and Scopus, 49 met inclusion criteria, focusing on dyslexia without neurological or psychiatric comorbidities. Article selection involved paired evaluation, with a third reviewer resolving discrepancies. The selected articles were analyzed using two topics: (1) a demographic and cognitive assessment of the sample and (2) DTI acquisition and analysis. Predominantly, studies centered on English-speaking children with reading difficulties, with preserved non-verbal intelligence, attention, and memory, and deficits in reading tests, rapid automatic naming, and phonological awareness. Structural differences were found mainly in the left AF in all ages and in the bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus for readers-children and adults. A better understanding of structural brain changes of dyslexia and neuroadaptations can be a guide for future interventions.

1. Introduction

Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that impairs fluency and/or speed of reading, as well as word recognition; it may also impact spelling. The difficulties should not be explained by other cognitive, health, or socio-economic factors [1]. The lack of accurate and fluent reading is a product of poor recognition and decoding abilities, and despite that, verbal comprehension is not equally affected in dyslexia [2,3].
Developmental dyslexia is classified within the framework of the diagnosis-specific learning disorder of reading [4], and it manifests in a spectrum from mild, to moderate, to severe [5]. It is present in over 80% of people with a learning disability in studied languages, having some variations regarding severity and type of difficulties according to the language structural characteristics [6]. Dyslexia’s mean prevalence is estimated at around 7% of the world population, with a predominance in male individuals [2,3]. Still, the incidence and prevalence are unclear due to the heterogeneity of literacy and language cultures with wide variances in terms of definitions, diagnostic instruments, rules, guidelines, and protocols for assessing dyslexic children and adults [1,7].
Two main medical classifications (ICD-11 and DSM-5) define the diagnostic criteria for dyslexia, and the assessment focuses mainly on identifying the reading and spelling discrepancies compared to the general population performance together with the assessment of other aspects that could confirm or rule out the diagnosis, for example, intelligence, phonological awareness, word and pseudoword reading, verbal fluency, verbal working memory, reading, and naming under stress [8]. This careful and time-consuming evaluation aims to support the clinical exclusion criteria, which contributes to reducing the prevalence by avoiding wrongful diagnoses of individuals struggling to read. Furthermore, there has been an effortful search in different fields for a better understanding of dyslexia’s development and neuroimaging contribution in clarifying neuroanatomical forming behind reading and dyslexia.
Clinical neuroimaging has been transformed by Diffusion-Weighted Imaging (DWI) and Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI), making it possible to examine the brain’s architecture and identify pathology earlier and more accurately than traditional magnetic resonance imaging sequences. Nowadays, diffusion is already used in clinical practice for stroke, trauma, tumors, demyelinating conditions, and neurosurgical planning. In psychiatric and neurological conditions, it is still used mainly in the research context [9].
The foundation of diffusion imaging lies in the behavior of water molecules, which move freely through space equally in all directions when unimpeded by structures. However, when encountering obstacles like cell membranes, water molecules tend to diffuse in alignment with the orientation of those barriers [10]. Magnetic resonance imaging, facilitated by DWI sequences, enables the measurement of water displacement in various directions for a brief duration. This information can then be used to assess tissue integrity, particularly in white matter fiber pathways [9].
Recent advancements in DTI research have expanded the focus beyond the assessment of a straightforward diffusion scalar to emphasize the significance of the more intricate 3D diffusion pattern. Axial diffusivity (AD), radial diffusivity (RD), mean diffusivity (MD), apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC), and others are additional imaging metrics that are increasingly used [11].
A standard DTI metric utilized in evaluating a variety of neuropathologic processes, from traumatic brain injury to demyelinating illness, is fractional anisotropy (FA), which quantifies the degree of this directionality [11]. Despite its promise for identifying subtle illnesses and alterations not discernible with conventional MRI sequences, the clinical applications of DTI have been questioned regarding the specificity of the findings [11].
In developmental neuroscience, DTI metrics have been shown to be a useful biomarker of white matter tract development and tissue injury, being used for treatment monitoring and potentially acting as outcome predictors. Children’s normal and pathological brain maturation has been described by the ADC/FA scalars since it is well known that as brain myelination and maturation advance, ADC values fall and FA values rise [12].
The literature has shown that people with dyslexia or reading disability may show brain structural changes with lower FA values in the left frontal and temporoparietal regions that coincide with the majority of studies on the left arcuate fasciculus (AF) and corona radiata (CR). Few studies have suggested a role for the posterior part of the corpus callosum or more ventral tracts like the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) or the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) [13]. And more recently, a meta-analysis found no differences between dyslexics and typical readers when observing studies that conducted Voxel-Based Analysis (VBA) of FA and compared it to reading ability [14].
The purpose of this systematic review is to search for a better understanding of the multitude of possible methods for analyzing DTI data while accounting for the characteristics of a clinical population such as developmental dyslexia in the literature in the last 10 years.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Search Strategy

The systematic review searched the primary databases, PubMed and Scopus, for publications published within the last ten years, including the period from January 2011 to September 2022. The indexed articles were selected, and their findings were reported, following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [15], and this study was not registered on Prospero. The criteria of interest selected were keywords in the following sequence: ((Dyslexia) AND (Brain connectivity) OR (Diffusion tensor imaging), using the boolean operators (DecS/MeSH):
SCOPUS: ((TITLE-ABS-KEY (dyslexia) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disorder”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disorders”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disability”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Reading disabilities” [) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disability”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disabilities”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disorder”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Developmental reading disorders”))) AND ((TITLE-ABS-KEY (dti) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tensor MRI”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tensor Magnetic Resonance Imaging”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (tractography) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tractography”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion Tensor Imaging”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“Diffusion weight imaging”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (dwi))) AND (LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “ar”)) AND (LIMIT-TO (LANGUAGE, “English”)) AND (LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2022) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2021) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2020) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2019) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2018) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2017) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2016) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2015) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2014) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2013) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2012) OR LIMIT-TO (PUBYEAR, 2011)).
PubMed: (((((((((Dyslexia*[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disorder”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disorders”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disability”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Reading disabilities”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disability”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disabilities”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disorder”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Developmental reading disorders”[Title/Abstract]) AND ((((((DTI[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tensor MRI”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tensor Magnetic Resonance Imaging”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tractography”[Title/Abstract]) OR “Diffusion Tensor Imaging”[Title/Abstract]) OR Tractography[Title/Abstract])).

2.2. Inclusion Criteria

The review included only original articles written in English published within the last 10 years, and full text available about dyslexia in structural analyses by DTI. According to the patient/problem, intervention, comparison, and outcome (PICO) criterion, the problem was: the structural brain changes in dyslexia are unclear; the intervention was: structural analysis by DTI; the comparison was: differences between dyslexia and typical readers volunteers; and the outcome was: the structural brain pattern of dyslexia of development.

2.3. Exclusion Criteria

We excluded studies based on the following criteria: (i) reviews or meta-analyses; (ii) publications written in languages other than English; (iii) indexed articles published in more than one database (duplicates); (iv) articles that included dyslexia with other neurological or psychiatric comorbidities such as stroke, brain injury, epilepsy, autism, traumatic brain injury, aphasia, and mood disorders; (v) articles that performed any analysis other than DTI, such as machine learning, graph theory, and only methodological comparison; (vi) articles in which the diagnosis of dyslexia is unclear; (vii) articles without at least one outcome or method of analysis reporting DTI measures and/or correlation with demographic or neuropsychological measures; (viii) case reports; (ix) neonates; and (x) dyslexia with genetic alterations.

2.4. Data Compilation

In this review, seven of the authors (B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C., A.S.C., K.L., and M.P.N.), in pairs, independently and randomly analyzed, reviewed, and assessed the eligibility of titles and abstracts according to the strategy of established search. The authors B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C., A.S.C., K.L., and M.P.N. selected the final articles by evaluating the texts that met the selection criteria. The authors B.M., E.M.D., L.F.C., and A.S.C. were responsible for the search for the demographic, clinical, and neuropsychological characteristics of volunteers and dyslexia patients, being checked by the senior authors (K.L. and M.P.N.). The authors B.M., M.Y.B., L.F.C., and A.S.C. searched for the characteristics of structural brain analyses and their outcomes, and all data were checked by the senior authors (K.L. and M.P.N.). All of the authors contributed to writing the entire text of this review.

2.5. Data Extraction

The selected articles were analyzed using two topics, which were represented in tables that addressed the following characteristics: (1) the demographic characteristics of the population sample, their language, their nationality, and the neuropsychological tools used to characterize the reading disorder; and (2) the characteristics of DTI acquisition, the parameters used in the image analyses and corrections, the structural outcomes between groups, and the correlation with clinical data when reported.

2.6. Risk of Bias Assessment

The selection of articles was performed in pairs, and a third independent author decided if the articles should be included. The data selected in the tables were divided by the authors into the groups already described above, and the checking of the data was carried out by the following group. The final inclusion of studies into the systematic review was by agreement of all reviewers.

2.7. Data Analysis

The data from the articles included in the tables were analyzed descriptively using the percentage, mean, and standard deviation; the variation to characterize each factor attributed to the demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of the participants in each study; and the characteristics of the acquisition, analysis, and results of the structural assessment performed by DTI image acquisition.

3. Results

3.1. Overview of the Screening Process of the Included Studies

Following the inclusion and exclusion criteria described above, we found 124 articles in the last ten years throughout the Scopus and PubMed databases, with 116 from Scopus and 8 from PubMed. Of the 116 articles found in Scopus, 64 were excluded after screening, two studies were with participants with alexia, one with aphasia, two with autism, one with mood disorder, two neonates, 20 without dyslexia diagnosis, two with genetic alterations, nine with brain injury (such as stroke, epilepsy, cortical lesion, and TBI), six were case reports, three were meta-analyses, seven were reviews, three were methodological studies, and six only featured morphometric analyses. The eligibility analysis excluded a further four articles. Three studies reported different methodologies of DTI analysis (machine learning, graph theory, and manual and automatic segmentation), and one did not report the DTI results, resulting in 48 studies included in this selection. Out of the eight articles identified in PubMed, five were excluded during screening: two lacked a dyslexia diagnosis, and three were duplicates from Scopus. After the eligibility assessment, an additional two studies were excluded. One study conducted DTI analysis using machine learning, while another did not report DTI results. Consequently, only one study from PubMed was included [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59] in this systematic review, 48 were included from Scopus, and one was included from PubMed, as shown in Figure 1.

3.2. Demographic and Neuropsychological Characteristics of Studied Dyslexia Subjects

This systematic review aimed to provide an overview of dyslexia in various stages of life, from early childhood (preschool to kindergarten) with a familiar history of dyslexia to elementary school children, adolescents, and adults, following the structural brain changes involved in this neurobiological disorder. In the selected studies about dyslexia and structural brain analysis by tractography included in this systematic review, 15% of studies [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23] included only children below 6 years old with the family risk of dyslexia before reading acquisition (classified as pre-readers); followed by 70% of the studies that analyzed older groups, at different stages of reading proficiency (classified as reading children) from 7 years old up to 19, males and females (classified as readers); and 15% of studied male and female adults aged from 20 to 33 years old (classified as reading adults) (Table 1).
Another important aspect is that dyslexia can manifest similarly across languages, but the characteristics and challenges may vary based on the language’s structure, writing system, and phonological rules. Because of this, we include the demographic characteristics of the country and the language participants spoke. The pre-reader studies were done mainly in the United States with English language speakers (50%) [16,17,18,22], followed by 40% [20,21,23] in Belgium with Dutch speakers, and 10% [19] in Germany with German speakers, including young children under 6 years old male and females and balanced distribution (a total of 193 females to 215 males) and with a sample variation of 10 to 46 children in each comparison group. In studies with reading-stage children, 47% spoke English—from the USA (45%) [25,27,31,32,33,39,42,44,48,50,52,53,54,56] and Canada (2%) [28,29]; 17% spoke French (the study was carried out in France [26,30,34,38,45]); 11% spoke Dutch—the studies were carried out in Belgium and Netherlands [37,41]; 8% were German; 6% spoke Mandarin—the studies were carried out in China [43] and Taiwan [36]; and 3% of studies were done with speakers of Arabic (Egypt) [35], Spanish (from Spain) [47], Portuguese (from Brazil) [49], or Italian (from Italy) [51]. In adults, the language of studies was less varied: 38% spoke English, and the studies were carried out in New Zealand [59] and the USA [62,64]; 25% spoke German (Germany) [58,60], 25% Dutch (Belgium) [61,63], and 12% Finnish (Finland) [57] (Table 1).
The neuropsychological characterization of the studied subjects followed the specificity of dyslexia diagnostic criteria, that the neuropsychological tests that were used included assessment of intelligence quotient (IQ); word and non-word reading and spelling tests; reading comprehension tests; rapid automatized naming (RAN); phonological awareness; and language, attention, and executive functions (description in Table 1). The primary purpose of the neuropsychological evaluation was to compare the performance of the dyslexic groups with the control group.
In the pre-reader, the intelligence was evaluated only by non-verbal tests, and only one study [16] showed a significant difference, with a worse performance of a risk of dyslexic children compared to the typically developing ones. One study did not report an intelligence assessment, possibly due to the very early age of participants [18]. In the reader children group, almost all studies evaluated the IQ (94.6%) with 51.4% by non-verbal and verbal IQ tests [25,26,27,30,31,38,39,40,45,47,48,49,52,53,54], 40.5% with only non-verbal tests [25,28,33,34,36,41,44,46,55,56], and 2.7% with only the verbal tests [50]. The significant difference between groups occurred in 34.3% of studies in verbal IQ tests and only 5.9% in non-verbal IQ tests. In the adult readers, 57.1% of the studies were administered non-verbal IQ tests [57,60,61,63,64] and only 28.6% showed significant differences with worse outcomes for dyslexic adults.
Concerning reading skills, in pre-readers, 89% [16,17,19,20,21,22,23] of studies assessed the children by word, letters, or pseudoword reading, and out of these 44% [16,17,19,21,22,23] had significantly lower outcome compared to the control group. In this age range, the RAN was also generally employed (78%) [16,17,19,20,22,23], with objects and colors being the most commonly used items (56%). A significant difference was found in naming speeds, mainly for slower naming of objects by dyslexic children in 44% of studies of this population. The phonological awareness was also mostly assessed (75%) [16,17,19,20,21,22,23], and in 25% of studies performance was lower in dyslexic groups. As for the other cognitive functions, attention was assessed in just one study [19], and the findings on working memory, digit span, visual reception, and gross and fine motor were also seldom reported.
Regarding the reader children, in all of the studies the reading skills were assessed by the word reading, and as would be expected lower performance of children with dyslexia compared to controls was found in 79% of studies [24,27,30,31,32,33,34,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. Other significant between-group differences with worse outcomes for dyslexic children were reported for pseudoword reading (70% [24,27,28,30,31,32,33,34,38,39,40,41,44,45,46,47,48,51,52,53,54,55,56] out of 85% assessed studies) and text reading (39% [24,30,32,34,39,40,45,46,51,52,54,55,56] out of 64% assessed studies). In 45% of all studies, the phonological awareness was assessed [24,26,27,31,34,35,38,39,40,41,43,45,46,51,53], and 39% of studies [24,26,27,31,34,38,39,40,41,43,45,46,51,53] produced significant results when compared between groups. When assessing RAN, 39% of studies evaluated this ability [24,26,29,34,35,38,40,41,43,45,46,52,56], and 33% found a significantly worse outcome for the dyslexic children when compared between groups [24,26,34,38,40,41,43,45,46,52,56]. A small number of studies (9%) assessed language [38,43,56] and attention [24,27,40]; however, almost all of these studies showed worse performance of subjects with dyslexia when compared to the controls. Half of the studies reported results of other cognitive functions in different aspects of short-term memory (digit span 33.3%) [24,26,27,38,40,45,46], verbal working memory (33.3%) [24,26,38,40,43,46], working memory (17%) [41,47], and arithmetic or mood behavior (11%) [46,56]. Almost all of these assessments also reported significant between-group differences and lower outcomes for participants with dyslexia.
For the reading adults, 100% of studies assessed volunteers for the word and/or pseudoword reading [57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64]. In 37.5% of the studies [57,58,62,64], text reading was tested, and all tests produced significant findings that allowed the groups to be distinguished based on the lower performance of the dyslexic group. Only 50% of these studies that tested RAN and phonological awareness showed significant group differences [57,59,63,64], and 25% assessed language and attention domains [57,59], without significant group differences.
Considering the neuropsychological outcome of reading children in the three language groups most represented in this revision (English, French, and Dutch), there was no difference in the cognitive skills found impaired in dyslexic volunteers compared to the controls.

3.3. Brain Structural Connectivity Characteristics on Acquisition, Process, and Outcomes of Dyslexia

The structural analysis through the DTI acquisition was performed in 88% of studies in high-field MRI equipment (3T) (scanner by manufacturers: Siemens (Berlin, Germany) (49%) [16,17,18,19,22,24,25,26,30,31,32,34,36,38,40,41,43,44,45,46,55,57,58,60], Philips (Eindhoven, Netherlands) (35%) [20,21,23,27,33,37,47,48,50,51,52,53,54,61,63,64], General Electric (GE, New York, United States of America) (4%)) [28,56] and 10% in low-field (1.5T) (scanner by manufacturers: Siemens (6%) [39,59,62], Philips or GE (2%) [35,49]) used in the acquisition of older participants such as reader children (more than 7 year old) and adults (more than 18 year old), as shown in Table 2.
Most of the selected studies (91.8%) used the sequence DTI for the diffusion analysis, and only 6.1% used diffusion kurtosis imaging (DKI) protocols [25,32,33] that require at least 3 b-values (as compared to 2 b-values for DTI) and at least 30 independent diffusion gradient directions (as compared to 6 for DTI), in which these 3 b-values were used: 0, 700 or 800 or 1000, and 2000 s/mm2 with 30 or 32, and 64 noncollinear diffusion directions. In two studies, the DTI sequence also was reported with 3 b-values (0, 700, or 1000, and 1000 or 2000 for b-values with 64 diffusion directions), 50% of DTI studies used 2 b-values (0 and 700 or 800 or 1000 or 1400) and 37% studies only one b-value (700 or 800 or 1000 or 1300 or 1400 or 5000). Regarding the number of noncollinear diffusion gradient directions in the studies that acquired DTI sequence, 41.3% reported more than 60 directions (28.3% was 60 and 2.2% was 128 directions), 34.8% used between 30 to 56 directions, 13% of studies used less than 30 directions (the smaller number of directions was 6), and 10.9% did not report this parameter [46] (Table 2).
The basic pulse sequence repetition time (TR) and echo time (TE) parameters ranged from 3000 to 14,000 ms and from 55 to 110 ms, respectively; the slice image ranged from 23 slices with 5 mm of thickness to 160 slices with around 1.7 mm to cover the entire brain, and the field of view (FOV) ranged from 208 to 282 mm.
The DTI analysis was performed in the selected studies by different softwares, and usually (82%) used more than one software to conduct all of the analysis. Most of the studies (63%) used the FSL software [18,19,23,24,25,26,28,30,31,32,33,38,39,40,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,52,53,55,58,59,60,61,63] and its different tools (Neurite orientation dispersion and the density imaging (NODDI) model, Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS), FDT, DTIFIT, PR0OBTRACKX, and BEDPOSTX, among others) associated or not with other software; 18% of studies [16,17,18,21,22,27,32,40,41] used the VistaLab developed at Stanford University that comprises different tools such as MrDiffusion and Automated Fiber Quantification (AFQ); 20% used ExploreDTI [20,23,24,29,37,38,43,45,61,63]; 16% used the TrackVis [18,20,23,24,26,37,43,45]; 12% DTIprep [16,17,18,22,44,50], 10% MRTrix [19,25,32,40,57]; 10% SPM [41,51,56,62,64]; 6% TRActs Constrained by UnderLying Anatomy (TRACULA) [33,44,52]; 4% DSI Studio [36,57]; and 2% BrainVoyager [34,51], the only commercial software; and few studies used PANDA, DIPY, Reproducible Objective Quantification Scheme (ROQS), TORTOISE, and TractSeg.
Artifacts in DWI acquisitions lead to errors in tensor estimation, and Eddy Current (EC) distortions and Head Motion (HM) are the two primary intrinsic DTI acquisition abnormalities that may obliterate the voxel-wise correlation across all the DWIs. Most of the selected studies (73%) reported in the pre-processing step comprise the EC and HM (with a cutoff from 1.5 mm to 6 mm) corrections. Other studies (10%) reported corrections to the image (EPI) distortions, 4% for the Gibbs artifact (truncation or ringing artifact) or Marchenko–Pastur Principal Component Analysis (MP-PCA), and only 14% did not report any correction in the preprocessing image step.
Some FSL tools were used for these corrections such as CATNAP (Coregistration, Adjustment, and Tensor-solving a Nicely Automated Program), which is a data processing pipeline for Philips PAR/REC Magnetic Resonance data files, performing motion correction for both diffusion and structural images using FSL FLIRT; it adjusts the diffusion gradient directions for scanner settings (i.e., slice angulation, slice orientation, etc.) and motion correction (i.e., the rotational component of the applied transformation) and computes tensor and derived quantities (FA, MD, colormaps, eigenvalues, etc). Also, the TOPUP tool of FSL is used to correct images of the susceptibility-induced distortions (fix EPI distortions), and QUAD (Quality Assessment for DMRI) for automatically performing image quality control (QC) at the single subject.
The tracking of DTI group analysis was normally reported as whole brain, tract, or ROI-based, from voxel-based, and 34.7% of studies reported as ROI-based methods [17,18,20,21,22,34,35,37,39,42,48,50,52,53,54,60,63], 26.5% as whole brain and ROI-based methods [16,19,23,27,28,29,30,31,33,41,43,45,47], 22.4% only whole brain or fiber tract-based analysis [24,25,26,32,36,38,40,44,46,57,64], and 12.2% as voxel-based analysis [49,51,55,58,59,62].
Regarding DTI quantitative analysis, 92% measured FA; of these, 29% also reported AD or RD [18,22,31,39,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,54,61], and 22% MD. In a few studies, other anisotropy metrics were used such as 4% relative anisotropy (RA) [42,50], 2% QA [57], or hindrance-modulated orientation anisotropy (HMOA) [45]. The MD measure was also described by directionally averaged mean diffusivity (Dav) [54,64] and extra-axonal mean diffusivity (MDe) [32], in 4% and 2% of studies, respectively. The ADC [35] or exponential apparent diffusion coefficient (eADC) metrics were reported in 2% of studies. Some White Matter Tract Integrity (WMTI) metrics from DKI were reported in 2% of studies, such as axonal water fraction (AWF) [32], intra-axonal diffusivity (Da) [32], or MDe [32], and NODDI metrics such as Neurite density index (NDI) and Orientation dispersion index (ODI) were reported in 4% of studies each [31,33].
Different types of atlas were reported in the selected studies to segment the cortical and white matter region; 54% used one of FSL atlas such as Julich-Brain Cytoarchitectonic Atlas, MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) Structural Atlas, JHU (Johns Hopkins University) ICBM-DTI-81 White-Matter Tractography Atlas, and Harvard-Oxford atlas; 19% used one of three FS’s atlas (Desikan–Killiany, Destrieux, and Desikan–Killiany–Tourville cortical atlas); 9% used native space; 7% used the automated anatomical atlas (AAL), the template for SPM, AFQ, ExploreDTI, and PANDA software; 3% used Talairach atlas; and 9% did not report this information.
Most of the studies (90%) specified the tracts/ROI used to explore the group comparison or association between structural data and demographic or neuropsychological data, the main tracts were AF (49%) [16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,25,27,32,34,35,37,40,41,43,44,45,47,51,59,61,63], inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) (41%) [17,22,25,27,32,34,37,39,40,41,42,43,44,46,48,49,50,51,53,62], inferior frontal-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) (39%) [20,23,25,32,34,36,37,39,40,41,43,45,47,49,50,51,62,63,64], superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) (37%) [16,17,22,25,27,31,32,35,39,40,41,42,44,45,47,50,51,56], 24% for corpus callosum (CC) [17,25,36,39,47,51,52,53,54,55,61,62], 18% for corticospinal (CS) tract [18,32,35,39,40,41,42,49,50], 12% for CR (including anterior and posterior parts—aCR and pCR) [35,39,55,59,62,64], 20% for uncinate fasciculus (UF) [24,25,32,37,39,40,41,43,49,50], and 16% for thalamic radiation (ThR) (including anterior and posterior parts- aThR and pThR) [32,36,40,41,49,50,52,62]. Forceps minor (FMn) and major were reported in 10% of studies [32,40,41,49,50], as well as the cingulum (CG) [31,40,42,49,50]; less frequently was also reported in 4% optical radiation (OR) [42,51], as well as cerebellar peduncles [36], internal capsule [62], temporal and temporo-occipital regions [19], frontal aslant tract (FAT) [24], primary auditory cortex, lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) [60], and inferior colliculus (2% each). 6% of studies reported only the atlas used [26,29,30], did not specify the tracts or ROIs, and 4% did not report this information [46,57].
Regarding the results of the structural analysis of the brain, 89% of the studies described this finding, with the predominance of a decrease in FA in the left hemisphere, when comparing the dyslexia group with the control group, and 10% of the studies did not show a significant difference between the groups [19,25,46,59,64], occurring mainly in the group of adults (25%) [59,64].
The tractography analyses of the pre-reader group reported by the selected studies were based on the FA changes according to the risk of familial history of dyslexia (FHD); 13% reported higher FA of right SLF [16] or CC [17] compared to the children with positive FHD than TR negative FHD, and 50% showed lower FA of the left AF (as also long portion) [18,21,22,23] and 13% in IFOF [20], as shown in Figure 2, highlighting the frequency in green color. Almost all of these regions also showed a positive correlation with age and some language tests and predicted decoding skills or reading impairment.
In the reading children, 42% of these studies reported a significant change in FA values (27% lower [27,38,42,43,44,45,47,49,55] and 15% higher values [40,41,48,52,54] in dyslexic children than the TR group), decreasing mainly in the left tracts such as AF, SLF, ILF, CR, and IFOF and increasing in the right SLF, aThR, cingulum, and CC. These studies also showed 6% of AD [43,50] decreased in dyslexic children in IFOF, SLF, UF, FMn, CG, and the right ThR [50], as well as in the left AF and ILF [43], and 12% of MD [28,32,39] changes between groups (6% higher [28,32] and 6% lower values in dyslexic children [32,39]). Only 3% of these studies reported higher values of ODI in the left ventral optical tract [33] of DYX and lower values of RD and HMOA (in UF of DYX males) [24], as shown in Figure 2, highlighting the frequency in red color. No group differences were reported in 6% of these studies [25,46], and 30% did not report brain changes [26,29,30,31,34,35,36,37,51,56]
In the selected studies with adult participants, 50% showed group differences, in which the DYX had low FA in lateral geniculate nucleus, TOC, and left-AF [60,63], and another DTI metric was the QA with high values for DYX group in left- UF, CS, CC, ThR, FMj, and parietal tracts and low in left SLF, CS, and OF in comparison to TR group [57], as shown in Figure 2, highlighting the frequency in purple color. In total, 25% of these studies did not find a group difference [59,64] or report this information [61,62].
The outcomes were also reported by the correlation analysis between clinical, demographic, and neuropsychological data, with white matter changes in 83.6% of studies, in which 60.9% showed a positive correlation between FA/MD changes and age (12%) [18,25,39,41,48,54]; gender (4%) [25,29], mainly in children; and neuropsychological tests (phonological awareness, word/no-word identification skill, VAS, working memory, verbal fluency, digit span, and Chinese character recognition) (44.9%) [20,22,24,25,27,29,30,31,32,35,36,37,38,39,43,44,46,54,56,57,61,63] in all ages; 32.6% reported a negative correlation between FA/MD/RD changes and neuropsychological performance [28,30,32,34,35,37,39,40,43,44,45,46,52,57,60,61]; 5% of studies reported [19,21,23] changes between white matter and DYX diagnoses or neuropsychological outcomes/skills; and 16.4% did not find or report correlation results [33,42,47,49,50,53,55,59].

4. Discussion

This systematic review provided an overview of the main structural brain changes findings by the DTI technique in developmental dyslexia in different age groups, including early at-risk children due to a family history of the disorder, as well as children and adults with reading disorders who have been diagnosed with dyslexia. By comparing the results of several studies, we describe converging evidence on structural abnormalities of the brain and the associations between imaging results and changes in this population’s characteristics. This systematic review observed that the assessment of different cognitive functions by the neuropsychological instruments of the selected studies varied according to age, country, and language; however, the lack of standardized procedures among age and language groups drastically reduces the possibility of comparing the behavioral outcomes and its relation to the structural brain changes. While some aspects of reading, mainly word decoding and recognition, are the most assessed skills in all the age groups, others such as language, attention, and working memory are reported exceptionally. The intelligence measure showed an expected outcome in terms of experimental and control group matching, with more studies reporting lower performance of children with dyslexia on verbal but not on non-verbal tests of intelligence. The intelligence assessment has generated a long-time debate in the field of learning, and while in typically developing children intelligence performance generally correlates with the achievement level, in reading impairment the intellectual disability is inconsistent with a diagnosis of dyslexia [65].
The importance of matching groups on intelligence measures is also supported by a growing body of evidence linking the brain white matter organization to intelligence level and processing efficiency. A recent study showed stronger integration of white matter structures within a local community (ex. frontal region) and especially with external brain networks (ex. frontal to parietal regions) in adults scoring high on non-verbal tests compared to average performers [66].
The structural brain changes shown by the DTI measurements were examined or extracted using the atlases, and nearly all studies (92%) examined the fractional anisotropy, which represents the directionality and organization of tissue microstructure; other studies examined some of its variants, including RA, QA, and HMOA, providing additional information to FA. The studies also examined the following measures: the AD measure, which can show changes in the density or integrity of axons within white matter pathways; the RD measure, whose increase frequently denotes disruptions in the microstructure of white matter, such as demyelination or axonal damage; and the MD measure, which, along with some variations like Dav and MDe, typically shows higher values that indicate decreased tissue integrity and increased diffusion.
In children before reading acquisition who had a positive family history of dyslexia, the structural changes analyzed by DTI metrics were shown mainly by the FA alterations. The decrease in FA was predominant in the left of the AF, as well as for IFOF, a result also reported in a recent study with dyslexic children with this profile [21], and a pattern of increased FA occurred in few studies, and only in the right hemisphere of the SLF and sCC, which may suggest possible early neural compensatory mechanisms in the right hemisphere [17]. This pattern was also similar in reading children, with low FA values mainly in the left hemisphere involving the AF, but also high FA values in the right hemisphere of AF, showing more neuroplasticity signals than the other young group. In addition, in these reader groups the structural changes covered more areas, still with a predominance of the left hemisphere, and were identified in other DTI metrics, such as low AD values in AF, SLF, IFOF, UF, ThR, ILF, CG, and CS, as well as a high MD values in AF, and low in UF and CR. Children submitted to reading intervention show an increase in MD values in AF and other reading brain circuitry such as the left ILF and posterior CC [67]. Variations of anisotropy, such as RA and HMOA, also showed low values in brain structural changes reported in the articles with reader children, but only FA showed high values in the following tracts AF, SLF, CC, ThR, ILF, and CG.
In dyslexic adults, the percentage of structural changes reported was much lower than those reported in children. It seems that with brain development, in adulthood, the structural differences of dyslexia become less evident due to neuroadaptation. However, the pattern of decreased FA measurements in dyslexic adults remained the same, with predominance in the left hemisphere of AF and the region that comprises the AF (lateral geniculate nucleus, and temporo-occipital cortex), as well as bilaterally SLF, CC, CS, and the left of UF and ThR by the QA measurements. This anisotropy variation, the QA measure, also showed an increase in the left side of SLF, VOF, and CS, which may represent neuroplasticity in adulthood. Nonetheless, due to literature scarcity on dyslexia in adults it is difficult to compare these results found in the review with other studies.
Another form of result widely explored between studies was the analysis of association, be it through correlation or the prediction of structural data with demographic or neuropsychological data, and this occurred with a greater incidence of significant findings between studies than the actual comparison of structural changes. These association analyses are normally applied to assist in the interpretation of results, mainly in studies with adults when there were no structural difference between the groups, but measures mainly of FA were positively or negatively correlated with the results of neuropsychological tests. In children, the structural changes helped predict some performance in neuropsychological tests, especially in children with a family risk of dyslexia.
In addition to the structural results found in participants with dyslexia and controls, this review considered how DTI data were acquired, processed, and extracted as an analysis. In general, all studies included in this review took good care to ensure good image and data quality, reducing bias in the interpretation of results.
The DTI acquired in high magnetic field equipment, such as 3 Tesla, can increase the capacity to acquire higher resolution scans more quickly, with higher b-values and thinner slices, as well as to increase tissue contrast and reduce background noise (thereby increasing the signal-to-noise ratio and contrast-to-noise ratio) [68]; this magnetic field was used in 88% of the studies (49% Siemens, 35% Philips, and 4% GE), in the acquisition of both DKI and DTI sequences to study tractography. The latter sequence is the more traditional sequence and was used in most of the selected articles in our systematic review. The difference between the two is that DKI significantly reduces the error of dODF orientation estimates compared to DTI and makes it possible to detect crossing fibers, which leads to a noticeable improvement in tractography across regions with complex fiber bundle geometries [69,70]. DKI-based tractography has potential benefits, especially in clinical contexts when time is of the essence [71].
The acquisition parameters of the DTI is important because they affect values of white matter (WM) scalar metrics, including FA, MD, Signal Noise Ratio (SNR), and even entire brain tractography investigations. These acquisition parameters include the diffusion sensitivity coefficient (b-value), which is a factor that reflects the strength and timing of those gradients used to generate DWI, as well as the reliability of DTI results concerning image and data quality; diffusion directions, in which there are more directions the longer the acquisition time; and voxel size (the smaller the size, the higher the quality [72,73]).
The adequate b-value for diffusion imaging quality evaluation in dyslexia was unclear and varied according to the equipment’s magnetic field. In a heath brain, a greater b-value usually results in a poorer SNR and image quality because increased signal attenuation owing to diffusion as well as increased TE (and therefore additional signal loss due to T2 decay), would lead to the decrease of MD, AD, and RD when the gradient directions and voxel resolution remained constant [73,74]. In the literature, this is more evident at the low field strength of the MR scanner as 1.5 T [75], and only 10% of the selected studies of this review used this field, less evident in high field strength (3 T and 7 T) due to their relatively high SNR, in which the increase in b value has little influence on the decrease in SNR, showing the best image and data quality with the respective b-values, 200 and 900 s/mm2 [73]. The selected studies included in this review reported normally more than one b-value and the MRI of 1.5T used b-values ranging from 800 to 1000 s/mm2 and the 3 T from 700 to 5000 s/mm2 [73,74].
The increased number of diffusion-encoding gradient directions can also improve DTI quality by averaging and strengthening the tensor estimation, and the opposite can reduce all DTI scalar values’ accuracy and precision; a minimum of 18 diffusion directions is advised to produce trustworthy DTI scalar results using the TBSS toolbox of FSL software [76]. In our study, just a small number of the selected studies (11.8%) used fewer than 30 directions, and of these, 7.8% used less than 18 directions, whereas the majority (78.4%) used 30 directions or more (41.3% used more than 60 directions).
Considering the impact of MRI acquisition parameters on the values of DTI measures, changes in the number of gradients and voxel resolution have the greatest impact on the FA, but variations in the b-value have a special effect on MD [72]. Another study also showed that the number of gradient directions was more relevant than the spatial resolution in some quantitative measures of DTI, such as tract volume, median fiber density, and mean FA, but this did not occur for all tracts evaluated in the same way, only for SLF and IFOF [77].
One of the most defining and defiant components of building a tracking algorithm is determining the underlying model that connects the raw dMRI images to the local fiber orientations, and presently, there is a wide range of software packages that incorporate higher-order fiber-tracking techniques that can calculate the relative contributions and orientations of several fiber populations within each voxel, which are easily applied to clinically relevant data sets [78].
A wide range of processing functions are offered by these software packages, such as tensor calculations, fiber tracking, visualization, statistical analysis, quantitative measure extraction from DTI datasets, and integration with additional neuroimaging tools. They vary, though, when it comes to the tractography algorithms that are applied, such as probabilistic, which produces a vast collection or distribution of potential trajectories from each seed point, and deterministic, which assumes a unique fiber orientation estimate in each voxel [79]; as well as the local approach, which is a fast and widely used method, it follows the local orientations of previously extracted fibers independently of each other, but the sum of small errors in these local orientations can significantly affect the final result, making it a very weak predictor of data with little quantitative significance or biological [78]. On the other hand, the global methods offer improved stability concerning noise and imaging artifacts and a greater agreement with the real dMRI data that was recorded; however, they rely on stochastic optimization approaches and hence do not guarantee convergence to a globally optimal solution [80].
The choice of software to analyze the DTI normally depends on the specific analysis requirements, familiarity with the software, and preference for user interface and workflow, and the studies normally used more than one software to employ specific tools to conduct the entire analysis.
Several image adjustments are usually conducted before DTI analysis to enhance the data quality and reduce common artifacts that may occur during data acquisition, allowing for appropriate interpretation and trustworthy outcomes. Most of the adjustments reported by the studies were Eddy current correction (74%) which aligns the DWI to a reference image acquired without diffusion weighting [81], and motion correction (74%); few explore structural analyses but they realign the image to compensate for subject motion, ensuring that the diffusion measurements are accurate and consistent across the dataset [82]. However, in low frequency the Gibbs ringing correction (4%) of the discontinuities in the k-space data caused by undersampling during image acquisition was also reported, resulting in obscure anatomical structures and affecting diffusion measurements, as well as EPI distortion correction (10%), considering the spatial variations in the strength and direction of the magnetic field gradients used for diffusion encoding. Unhappily, 14% of the studies did not report any adjustment before DTI analyses.
Regarding the type of quantitative DTI analyses reported by the studies, most (69.2%) used voxel-wise, which analyzes each voxel to identify differences in diffusion properties between groups or conditions, providing more detailed spatial information about diffusion metrics at a voxel-level; this is present in some of the software such as FSL, SPM, MRItrix, and ExploreDTI. Interestingly, a lot of articles reported this analysis as the whole brain since voxel-based analysis includes all the cerebro voxels. Also, ROI-based analysis used in 61.5% of studies is frequently conducted after voxel-based, making it challenging to determine which method was used in each study.
The outcome description of the studies was based on anatomical regions of interest or specific tracts outlined by a well-established atlas that facilitates the interpretation of imaging data by providing standardized anatomical labeling and spatial coordinates. While the atlases share the goal of delineating brain structures and regions, they differ in several aspects, including their origins, resolutions, and intended applications.
The study’s limitations stem mainly from the differences in how dyslexia was defined across studies. The use of the terms dyslexia, developmental dyslexia, reading disorders, or never reading difficulties point to the lack of a common terminology and diagnosis criteria. The impact of the findings of this study could be that the results may be possibly drawn from a very heterogeneous sample. However, the cognitive testing of participants may ameliorate this and emerge as a potential tool mainly in internationally normed tests.
Another limitation of the study was the influence of language on dyslexia’s reading acquisition history. It is a well-known fact that readers in irregular language systems have longer reading acquisition and struggling readers may develop different compensation strategies.
Studies on neuroimaging may be limited by variations in image acquisition parameterization; however, despite this wide range, all studies used optimal acquisition and analysis parameters that did not affect the comparability of results, even in cases where certain fundamental information was not stated. In terms of results, a certain study focused on describing the anatomical regions examined rather than the tract as most studies did, taking into account the tracts involved in the regions described.

5. Conclusions

This systematic review of structural alterations in the brain associated with dyslexia revealed that over the past ten years studies on children have outnumbered those on adults, primarily focusing on boys and the English language. The studies also showed that brain changes concentrated in FA reduction in the fasciculus arcuate of the left hemisphere at all ages, and in the left superior longitudinal fascicle for reading in children and adults, as well as an increase in the right hemisphere, which may indicate signs of neuroadaptation. A better understanding of structural brain changes of dyslexia and neuroadaptations can be a guide for future interventions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization—B.M., K.L. and M.P.N.; methodology and validation—B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C., K.L. and M.P.N.; investigation and formal analysis—B.M., M.Y.B., E.M.D., L.F.C. and A.S.C.; writing—-original draft preparation, B.M., M.Y.B., K.L. and M.P.N.; writing—review and editing, B.M., K.L. and M.P.N.; and supervision and project administration—M.P.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES)—Programa de Demanda Social (code 001).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; Text Rev.; American Psychiatric Association Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 2012, 379, 1997–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. Developmental dyslexia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2015, 11, 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Peterson, R.L.; McGrath, L.M.; Willcutt, E.G.; Keenan, J.M.; Olson, R.K.; Pennington, B.F. How specific are learning disabilities? J. Learn. Disabil. 2021, 54, 466–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Habib, M. The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia and related disorders: A reappraisal of the temporal hypothesis, twenty years on. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Majeed, N.M.; Hartanto, A.; Tan, J.J. Developmental dyslexia and creativity: A meta-analysis. Dyslexia 2021, 27, 187–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Di Folco, C.; Guez, A.; Peyre, H.; Ramus, F. Epidemiology of developmental dyslexia: A comparison of DSM-5 and ICD-11 criteria. MedRxiv 2020, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Roitsch, J.; Watson, S.M. An overview of dyslexia: Definition, characteristics, assessment, identification, and intervention. Sci. J. Educ. 2019, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Van Hecke, W.; Emsell, L.; Sunaert, S. Diffusion Tensor Imaging: A Practical Handbook; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Beaulieu, C. The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—A technical review. NMR Biomed. Int. J. Devoted Dev. Appl. Magn. Reson. In Vivo 2002, 15, 435–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Steven, A.J.; Zhuo, J.; Melhem, E.R. Diffusion kurtosis imaging: An emerging technique for evaluating the microstructural environment of the brain. Am. J. Roentgenol. 2014, 202, W26–W33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Huisman, T. Diffusion-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging of the brain, made easy. Cancer Imaging 2010, 10, S163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Vandermosten, M.; Boets, B.; Wouters, J.; Ghesquière, P. A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1532–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Moreau, D.; Stonyer, J.E.; McKay, N.S.; Waldie, K.E. No evidence for systematic white matter correlates of dyslexia: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2018, 1683, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Int. J. Surg. 2021, 88, 105906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Zuk, J.; Dunstan, J.; Norton, E.; Yu, X.; Ozernov-Palchik, O.; Wang, Y.; Hogan, T.P.; Gabrieli, J.D.; Gaab, N. Multifactorial pathways facilitate resilience among kindergarteners at risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal behavioral and neuroimaging study. Dev. Sci. 2021, 24, e12983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Yu, X.; Zuk, J.; Perdue, M.V.; Ozernov-Palchik, O.; Raney, T.; Beach, S.D.; Norton, E.S.; Ou, Y.; Gabrieli, J.D.; Gaab, N. Putative protective neural mechanisms in prereaders with a family history of dyslexia who subsequently develop typical reading skills. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 41, 2827–2845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Langer, N.; Peysakhovich, B.; Zuk, J.; Drottar, M.; Sliva, D.D.; Smith, S.; Becker, B.L.; Grant, P.E.; Gaab, N. White matter alterations in infants at risk for developmental dyslexia. Cereb. Cortex 2017, 27, 1027–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kraft, I.; Schreiber, J.; Cafiero, R.; Metere, R.; Schaadt, G.; Brauer, J.; Neef, N.E.; Müller, B.; Kirsten, H.; Wilcke, A.; et al. Predicting early signs of dyslexia at a preliterate age by combining behavioral assessment with structural MRI. Neuroimage 2016, 143, 378–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Vandermosten, M.; Vanderauwera, J.; Theys, C.; De Vos, A.; Vanvooren, S.; Sunaert, S.; Wouters, J.; Ghesquière, P. A DTI tractography study in pre-readers at risk for dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015, 14, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Van Der Auwera, S.; Vandermosten, M.; Wouters, J.; Ghesquière, P.; Vanderauwera, J. A three-time point longitudinal investigation of the arcuate fasciculus throughout reading acquisition in children developing dyslexia. NeuroImage 2021, 237, 118087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Wang, Y.; Mauer, M.V.; Raney, T.; Peysakhovich, B.; Becker, B.L.; Sliva, D.D.; Gaab, N. Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls. Cereb. Cortex 2017, 27, 2469–2485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Vanderauwera, J.; Wouters, J.; Vandermosten, M.; Ghesquière, P. Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 27, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Zhao, J.; Song, Z.; Zhao, Y.; de Schotten, M.T.; Altarelli, I.; Ramus, F. White matter connectivity in uncinate fasciculus accounts for visual attention span in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2022, 177, 108414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Meisler, S.L.; Gabrieli, J.D. A large-scale investigation of white matter microstructural associations with reading ability. NeuroImage 2022, 249, 118909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Liu, T.; Thiebaut de Schotten, M.; Altarelli, I.; Ramus, F.; Zhao, J. Neural dissociation of visual attention span and phonological deficits in developmental dyslexia: A hub-based white matter network analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2022, 43, 5210–5219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Farah, R.; Glukhovsky, N.; Rosch, K.; Horowitz-Kraus, T. Structural white matter characteristics for working memory and switching/inhibition in children with reading difficulties: The role of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus. Netw. Neurosci. 2022, 6, 897–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Partanen, M.; Kim, D.H.; Rauscher, A.; Siegel, L.S.; Giaschi, D.E. White matter but not grey matter predicts change in reading skills after intervention. Dyslexia 2021, 27, 224–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lou, C.; Cross, A.M.; Peters, L.; Ansari, D.; Joanisse, M.F. Rich-club structure contributes to individual variance of reading skills via feeder connections in children with reading disabilities. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2021, 49, 100957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Liu, T.; de Schotten, M.T.; Altarelli, I.; Ramus, F.; Zhao, J. Maladaptive compensation of right fusiform gyrus in developmental dyslexia: A hub-based white matter network analysis. Cortex 2021, 145, 57–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Koirala, N.; Perdue, M.V.; Su, X.; Grigorenko, E.L.; Landi, N. Neurite density and arborization is associated with reading skill and phonological processing in children. NeuroImage 2021, 241, 118426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Huber, E.; Mezer, A.; Yeatman, J.D. Neurobiological underpinnings of rapid white matter plasticity during intensive reading instruction. NeuroImage 2021, 243, 118453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Borghesani, V.; Wang, C.; Watson, C.; Bouhali, F.; Caverzasi, E.; Battistella, G.; Bogley, R.; Yabut, N.A.; Deleon, J.; Miller, Z.A.; et al. Functional and morphological correlates of developmental dyslexia: A multimodal investigation of the ventral occipitotemporal cortex. J. Neuroimaging 2021, 31, 962–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Vander Stappen, C.; Dricot, L.; Van Reybroeck, M. RAN training in dyslexia: Behavioral and brain correlates. Neuropsychologia 2020, 146, 107566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. El-Sady, S.; Mohammad, S.A.; Aboualfotouh Ahmed, K.; Khattab, A.N.; Nashaat, N.H.; Orabi, G.; Abdelraouf, E.R. Correlation between diffusion tensor imaging measures and the reading and cognitive performance of Arabic readers: Dyslexic children perspective. Neuroradiology 2020, 62, 525–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Wang, H.L.S.; Wang, N.Y.H.; Yeh, F.C. Specifying the diffusion MRI connectome in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia and auditory processing deficits. Pediatr. Neonatol. 2019, 60, 297–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Vanderauwera, J.; van Setten, E.R.; Maurits, N.M.; Maassen, B.A. The interplay of socio-economic status represented by paternal educational level, white matter structure and reading. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0215560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lou, C.; Duan, X.; Altarelli, I.; Sweeney, J.A.; Ramus, F.; Zhao, J. White matter network connectivity deficits in developmental dyslexia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 40, 505–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lebel, C.; Benischek, A.; Geeraert, B.; Holahan, J.; Shaywitz, S.; Bakhshi, K.; Shaywitz, B. Developmental trajectories of white matter structure in children with and without reading impairments. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 36, 100633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Banfi, C.; Koschutnig, K.; Moll, K.; Schulte-Körne, G.; Fink, A.; Landerl, K. White matter alterations and tract lateralization in children with dyslexia and isolated spelling deficits. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 40, 765–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Žarić, G.; Timmers, I.; Gerretsen, P.; Fraga González, G.; Tijms, J.; van der Molen, M.W.; Blomert, L.; Bonte, M. Atypical white matter connectivity in dyslexic readers of a fairly transparent orthography. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Yagle, K.; Richards, T.; Askren, K.; Mestre, Z.; Beers, S.; Abbott, R.; Nagy, W.; Boord, P.; Berninger, V. Relationships between eye movements during sentence reading comprehension, word spelling and reading, and DTI and fMRI connectivity in students with and without dysgraphia or dyslexia. J. Syst. Integr. Neurosci. 2017, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Su, M.; Zhao, J.; de Schotten, M.T.; Zhou, W.; Gong, G.; Ramus, F.; Shu, H. Alterations in white matter pathways underlying phonological and morphological processing in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 31, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Christodoulou, J.A.; Murtagh, J.; Cyr, A.; Perrachione, T.K.; Chang, P.; Halverson, K.; Hook, P.; Yendiki, A.; Ghosh, S.; Gabrieli, J.D. Relation of white-matter microstructure to reading ability and disability in beginning readers. Neuropsychology 2017, 31, 508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Zhao, J.; de Schotten, M.T.; Altarelli, I.; Dubois, J.; Ramus, F. Altered hemispheric lateralization of white matter pathways in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from spherical deconvolution tractography. Cortex 2016, 76, 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Koerte, I.K.; Willems, A.; Muehlmann, M.; Moll, K.; Cornell, S.; Pixner, S.; Steffinger, D.; Keeser, D.; Heinen, F.; Kubicki, M.; et al. Mathematical abilities in dyslexic children: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016, 10, 781–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Garcia-Zapirain, B.; Garcia-Chimeno, Y.; Saralegui, I.; Fernandez-Ruanova, B.; Martinez, R. Differences in effective connectivity between children with dyslexia, monocular vision and typically developing readers: A DTI study. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 2016, 23, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Fernandez, V.G.; Juranek, J.; Romanowska-Pawliczek, A.; Stuebing, K.; Williams, V.J.; Fletcher, J.M. White matter integrity of cerebellar-cortical tracts in reading impaired children: A probabilistic tractography study. Brain Lang. 2016, 161, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. De Moura, L.M.; Cogo-Moreira, H.; De Ávila, C.R.B.; Pan, P.M.; Gadelha, A.; Moriyama, T.; Del Aquilla, M.A.; Hoexter, M.; Salum, G.A.; Picon, F.A.; et al. Children with poor reading skills at the word level show reduced fractional anisotropy in white matter tracts of both hemispheres. Brain Connect. 2016, 6, 519–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Richards, T.L.; Grabowski, T.J.; Boord, P.; Yagle, K.; Askren, M.; Mestre, Z.; Robinson, P.; Welker, O.; Gulliford, D.; Nagy, W.; et al. Contrasting brain patterns of writing-related DTI parameters, fMRI connectivity, and DTI–fMRI connectivity correlations in children with and without dysgraphia or dyslexia. Neuroimage Clin. 2015, 8, 408–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Marino, C.; Scifo, P.; Della Rosa, P.A.; Mascheretti, S.; Facoetti, A.; Lorusso, M.L.; Giorda, R.; Consonni, M.; Falini, A.; Molteni, M.; et al. The DCDC2/intron 2 deletion and white matter disorganization: Focus on developmental dyslexia. Cortex 2014, 57, 227–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Fan, Q.; Davis, N.; Anderson, A.W.; Cutting, L.E. Thalamo-cortical connectivity: What can diffusion tractography tell us about reading difficulties in children? Brain Connect. 2014, 4, 428–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Fan, Q.; Anderson, A.W.; Davis, N.; Cutting, L.E. Structural connectivity patterns associated with the putative visual word form area and children’ s reading ability. Brain Res. 2014, 1586, 118–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hasan, K.M.; Molfese, D.L.; Walimuni, I.S.; Stuebing, K.K.; Papanicolaou, A.C.; Narayana, P.A.; Fletcher, J.M. Diffusion tensor quantification and cognitive correlates of the macrostructure and microstructure of the corpus callosum in typically developing and dyslexic children. NMR Biomed. 2012, 25, 1263–1270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Gebauer, D.; Enzinger, C.; Kronbichler, M.; Schurz, M.; Reishofer, G.; Koschutnig, K.; Kargl, R.; Purgstaller, C.; Fazekas, F.; Fink, A. Distinct patterns of brain function in children with isolated spelling impairment: New insights. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 1353–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Hoeft, F.; McCandliss, B.D.; Black, J.M.; Gantman, A.; Zakerani, N.; Hulme, C.; Lyytinen, H.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Glover, G.H.; Reiss, A.L.; et al. Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 361–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Sihvonen, A.J.; Virtala, P.; Thiede, A.; Laasonen, M.; Kujala, T. Structural white matter connectometry of reading and dyslexia. NeuroImage 2021, 241, 118411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Tschentscher, N.; Ruisinger, A.; Blank, H.; Díaz, B.; Von Kriegstein, K. Reduced structural connectivity between left auditory thalamus and the motion-sensitive planum temporale in developmental dyslexia. J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 1720–1732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Moreau, D.; Wilson, A.J.; McKay, N.S.; Nihill, K.; Waldie, K.E. No evidence for systematic white matter correlates of dyslexia and dyscalculia. NeuroImage Clin. 2018, 18, 356–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Müller-Axt, C.; Anwander, A.; von Kriegstein, K. Altered structural connectivity of the left visual thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, 3692–3698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Vandermosten, M.; Poelmans, H.; Sunaert, S.; Ghesquière, P.; Wouters, J. White matter lateralization and interhemispheric coherence to auditory modulations in normal reading and dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 2087–2099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Lebel, C.; Shaywitz, B.; Holahan, J.; Shaywitz, S.; Marchione, K.; Beaulieu, C. Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of reading ability in dysfluent and non-impaired readers. Brain Lang. 2013, 125, 215–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Vandermosten, M.; Boets, B.; Poelmans, H.; Sunaert, S.; Wouters, J.; Ghesquiere, P. A tractography study in dyslexia: Neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing. Brain 2012, 135, 935–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Frye, R.E.; Liederman, J.; Hasan, K.M.; Lincoln, A.; Malmberg, B.; McLean, J., III; Papanicolaou, A. Diffusion tensor quantification of the relations between microstructural and macrostructural indices of white matter and reading. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011, 32, 1220–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Mather, N.; Schneider, D. The Use of Cognitive Tests in the Assessment of Dyslexia. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Bruzzone, S.; Lumaca, M.; Brattico, E.; Vuust, P.; Kringelbach, M.; Bonetti, L. Dissociated brain functional connectivity of fast versus slow frequencies underlying individual differences in fluid intelligence: A DTI and MEG study. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 4746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Huber, E.; Donnelly, P.M.; Rokem, A.; Yeatman, J.D. Rapid and widespread white matter plasticity during an intensive reading intervention. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 2260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Buchanan, C.R.; Muñoz Maniega, S.; Valdés Hernández, M.C.; Ballerini, L.; Barclay, G.; Taylor, A.M.; Russ, T.C.; Tucker-Drob, E.M.; Wardlaw, J.M.; Deary, I.J.; et al. Comparison of structural MRI brain measures between 1.5 and 3 T: Data from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2021, 42, 3905–3921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Lazar, M.; Jensen, J.H.; Xuan, L.; Helpern, J.A. Estimation of the orientation distribution function from diffusional kurtosis imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Int. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 2008, 60, 774–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Glenn, G.R.; Helpern, J.A.; Tabesh, A.; Jensen, J.H. Optimization of white matter fiber tractography with diffusional kurtosis imaging. NMR Biomed. 2015, 28, 1245–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Glenn, G.R.; Kuo, L.W.; Chao, Y.P.; Lee, C.Y.; Helpern, J.A.; Jensen, J.H. Mapping the orientation of white matter fiber bundles: A comparative study of diffusion tensor imaging, diffusional kurtosis imaging, and diffusion spectrum imaging. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2016, 37, 1216–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Barrio-Arranz, G.; de Luis-García, R.; Tristán-Vega, A.; Martín-Fernández, M.; Aja-Fernández, S. Impact of MR acquisition parameters on DTI scalar indexes: A tractography based approach. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0137905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Bao, S.S.; Zhao, C.; Bao, X.X.; Rao, J.S.; Rao, J. Effect of Value on Imaging Quality for Diffusion Tensor Imaging of the Spinal Cord at Ultrahigh Field Strength. BioMed Res. Int. 2021, 2021, 4836804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Qi, L.P.; Zhong, Z.; Sun, Y.S.; Li, X.T.; Tang, L.; Zhou, X.J. Optimal selection of b-values for differential diagnosis of mediastinal lymph nodes using diffusion-weighted imaging. Heliyon 2023, 9, e16702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Sartoretti, T.; Sartoretti, E.; Wyss, M.; Mannil, M.; van Smoorenburg, L.; Eichenberger, B.; Reischauer, C.; Alfieri, A.; Binkert, C.; Sartoretti-Schefer, S. Diffusion-weighted MRI of ischemic stroke at 3T: Value of synthetic b-values. Br. J. Radiol. 2021, 94, 20200869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Kumpulainen, V.; Merisaari, H.; Copeland, A.; Silver, E.; Pulli, E.P.; Lewis, J.D.; Saukko, E.; Saunavaara, J.; Karlsson, L.; Karlsson, H.; et al. Effect of number of diffusion-encoding directions in diffusion metrics of 5-year-olds using tract-based spatial statistical analysis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2022, 56, 4843–4868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Hoefnagels, F.W.; de Witt Hamer, P.C.; Pouwels, P.J.; Barkhof, F.; Vandertop, W.P. Impact of gradient number and voxel size on diffusion tensor imaging tractography for resective brain surgery. World Neurosurg. 2017, 105, 923–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Jeurissen, B.; Descoteaux, M.; Mori, S.; Leemans, A. Diffusion MRI fiber tractography of the brain. NMR Biomed. 2019, 32, e3785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Schlaier, J.R.; Beer, A.L.; Faltermeier, R.; Fellner, C.; Steib, K.; Lange, M.; Greenlee, M.W.; Brawanski, A.T.; Anthofer, J.M. Probabilistic vs. deterministic fiber tracking and the influence of different seed regions to delineate cerebellar-thalamic fibers in deep brain stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017, 45, 1623–1633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Christiaens, D.; Reisert, M.; Dhollander, T.; Sunaert, S.; Suetens, P.; Maes, F. Global tractography of multi-shell diffusion-weighted imaging data using a multi-tissue model. Neuroimage 2015, 123, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Bodammer, N.; Kaufmann, J.; Kanowski, M.; Tempelmann, C. Eddy current correction in diffusion-weighted imaging using pairs of images acquired with opposite diffusion gradient polarity. Magn. Reson. Med. Off. J. Int. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 2004, 51, 188–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Oldham, S.; Arnatkevičiūtė, A.; Smith, R.; Tiego, J.; Bellgrove, M.; Fornito, A. The efficacy of different preprocessing steps in reducing motion-related confounds in diffusion MRI connectomics. NeuroImage 2020, 222, 117252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart of this systematic review study, identifying at each stage the number of studies included and the reasons for excluding studies until the final stage of inclusion of studies.
Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart of this systematic review study, identifying at each stage the number of studies included and the reasons for excluding studies until the final stage of inclusion of studies.
Brainsci 14 00349 g001
Figure 2. Spider graphic of the diffusion tensor image (DTI) outcomes percentage distributed by the main tracts reported in the systematic review and their DTI metrics behavior found according to tract and age group of dyslexia participants (pre-reader children in green, reader children in red, and reader adults in purple). The arrows indicate the increase or decrease in DTI metrics in the dyslexia group in comparison to the control group. Abbreviations: AF: arcuate fasciculus; SLF: superior longitudinal fasciculus; IFOF: inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; CC: corpus callosum; UF: uncinate fasciculus; ThR: thalamic radiations; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; CG: cingulate cortex; CS: corticospinal fasciculus; CR: corona radiata; OR: optic radiation; Forceps: forceps major and minor; VOF: ventral occipital fasciculus; FA: fractional anisotropy; AD: axial diffusivity; MD: mean diffusivity; QA: quantitative anisotropy; RA: relative anisotropy; and HMOA: hindrance-modulated oriented anisotropy.
Figure 2. Spider graphic of the diffusion tensor image (DTI) outcomes percentage distributed by the main tracts reported in the systematic review and their DTI metrics behavior found according to tract and age group of dyslexia participants (pre-reader children in green, reader children in red, and reader adults in purple). The arrows indicate the increase or decrease in DTI metrics in the dyslexia group in comparison to the control group. Abbreviations: AF: arcuate fasciculus; SLF: superior longitudinal fasciculus; IFOF: inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; CC: corpus callosum; UF: uncinate fasciculus; ThR: thalamic radiations; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; CG: cingulate cortex; CS: corticospinal fasciculus; CR: corona radiata; OR: optic radiation; Forceps: forceps major and minor; VOF: ventral occipital fasciculus; FA: fractional anisotropy; AD: axial diffusivity; MD: mean diffusivity; QA: quantitative anisotropy; RA: relative anisotropy; and HMOA: hindrance-modulated oriented anisotropy.
Brainsci 14 00349 g002
Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological assessment.
Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological assessment.
Ref.YearCountryLanguageGroupNSex
(F:M)
Age
(Years)
Years of
Education
(or Level)
IQA Word Reading/SpellingPseudoword
Reading
Text
Reading
RANPhonological
Awareness
LanguageAttentionOthers
Zuk J, et al. [16]2021USAEnglishTR FHD−
TR FHD+
RD FHD+
39
18
17
21:18
7:11
9:8
5.5 ± 0.3
5.5 ± 0.3
5.7 ± 0.4
Pre-Kindergarten;
Kindergarten
Non-verbal (KBIT-2)LWID (WRMT-R/NU and WRMT-R); Letter Sound Knowledge (YARC)NRNRObjects;
Colors; Letter
Elision;
Blending Words
(CTOPP)
Vocabulary Knowledge (PPVT-4); Sentence Comprehension (CELF-4); Speed AccuracyNRWM: Nonword Repetition (CTOPP), Sentence Repetition (GAPS)
Yu X, et al. [17]2020USAEnglishTR FHD−
TR FHD+
RD FHD+
34
35
12
16:18
17:18
4:8
5.4 ± 0.3
5.5 ± 0.4
5.8 ± 0.5
The end of
1st grade to
4th/ grade
Non-verbal
(KBIT-2)
WID (WRMT-R);
SWE (TOWRE-2)
PDE (TOWRE-2);
WA (WRMT-R)
NRObjects; ColorsCTOPPCELF-4NRHLE
Langer N, et al. [18]2017USAEnglishFHD+
FHD−
14
18
7:7
10:8
0.9 ± 0.3
0.8 ± 0.3
NRNRNANANANANAExpressive and
receptive language
(MSEL)
NAGross and
fine motor (MSEL);
Visual reception
(MSEL)
Kraft I, et al. [19]2016GermanyGermanFHD+
FHD−
25
28
11:14
12:16
5.7 ± 0.4
5.6 ± 0.4
KindergartenNon-verbalOne minute
word reading (SLRT-II);
Spelling (DERET)
One minute
pseudoword reading
(SLRT-II)
NRSubtest (BISC)Pseudoword repetition
(SETK 3-5),
SS and RI (BISC), and
PA (BAKO)
NRSymbol comparison
(BISC)
DS (K-ABC)
Vandermosten M, et al. [20]2015BelgiumDutchFRD+
FRD−
36
35
13:23
17:18
5.1 ± 0.2
5.1 ± 0.2
The last
year of
kindergarten
Non-verbal
(Raven)
Letter knowledge
productive/receptive
test
NRNRObjects;
Colors
End-phoneme
and end-rhyme
identification
task (PA)
NRNRNR
Van Der Auwera S, et al. [21]2021BelgiumDutchPreR: FRD−
FRD+
BR: FRD−
FRD+
FR: FRD−
FRD+
24
16
13
24
10
15
14:10
8:8
13:10
12:12
2:8
5:10
6 ± 0.1
6 ± 0.1
8 ± 0.1
8 ± 0.1
11 ± 0.1
11 ± 0.2
Kindergarten1st/
2nd grades
3rd/4th/5th grades
Non-verbal
(Raven and
Block Design–
WISC-III)
Word Reading
List; Letter
knowledge; and
Spelling
Pseudoword Reading TestNRNRPhoneme Deletion and Spoonerism; PANRNRNR
Wang Y, et al. [22]2017USAEnglishPreR: FHD−
FHD+
BR: FHD−
FHD+
FR: FHD−
FHD+
16
24
23
24
10
15
8:8
14:10
12:12
13:10
2:8
5:10
5.3 ± 0.8
5.4 ± 1.1
7.0 ± 2.2
7.3 ± 2.2
10.0 ± 2.5
10.2 ± 1.8
Nine single words
1st/2nd grades
3rd/4th/5th grades
Non-verbal
(KBIT-2)
WID (WRMT-R)
(FR); SWE (TOWRE)
(BR and FR); and
TOSWRF (FR)
PDE
(TOWRE)
Gray Oral Reading Test(GORT-5), Reading Fluency WJ-III-TAObjects (PreR);
Colors
CTOPP (FR);
PC (WRMT-R, BR)
CELF-4 (BR)NRTOMAL2 (FR)
Vanderauwera J, et al. [23]2017BelgiumDutchDYX
TR
FRD+
FRD−
15
46
34
27
7:8
15:31
13:21
9:18
7.9 ± 0.1
7.9 ± 0.1
7.9 ± 0.1
7.9 ± 0.1
PreR—prior 1st grade/
BR—2nd/3rd grade
Non-verbal
(WISC)
Word reading, one-minute test (BR); Spelling (BR); and Productive/Receptive Letter Knowledge (PreR)Pseudoword reading
two minute
test (BR)
NRObjects (PreR);
Colors
PA (PreR);
End-phoneme and
end-rhyme
identification task
NANANR
Zhao J, et al. [24]2022FranceFrenchControl
DYX
31
26
13:18
13:13
12 ± 1:11 ± 2
11 ± 1:12 ± 1
NRVerbal (WISC);
Non-verbal (WISC)
Word Reading
Ability (Odedys);
Word Spelling-to-
Dictation Test
Nonword Reading
Ability (Odedys)
Alouette
Test
Digits; ObjectsPhoneme deletion
and Spoonerism
NRVAS (Global
and Partial
Letter Report Task)
Verbal WM
(DS- WISC)
Meisler SL, [25]2022USAEnglishControl
DYX
582
104
195:387
44:60
10.8 ± 3.2
10.2 ± 2.5
NRNon-verbal
(KBIT-2)
SWE
(TOWRE-2)
PDE
(TOWRE-2)
NRNRNRNRNRNR
Liu T, et al. [26]2022FranceFrenchControl
DYX
31
16
13:18
13:13
11 ± 1
12 ± 1
NRVerbal and Non-
verbal (WISC-IV)
NR; Global and
Partial Letter
Report Task (VAS)
NRNRDigits; ObjectsPhoneme deletion
and Spoonerism
NRNRVerbal WM (DS- WISC)
Farah R, et al. [27]2022USAEnglishControl
RD
24
22
12:12
10:12
8-12NRNon-verbal (TONI);
Verbal (PPVT)
SWE (TOWRE);
LWID (WJ-III)
PDE (TOWRE);
WA (WJ-III)
NRNRElision
(CTOPP)
NRConners questionnaires and VSA (TEA-Ch)DS (WISC); Switching/Inhibition (DKEFS, Color-Word Condition); and Overall EF (BRIEF)
Partanen M, et al. [28]2020CanadaEnglishControl
DYX
22
13
11:11
5:8
Pre-test
8.5 ± 0.4
8.6 ± 0.4
Post-test
8.9 ± 0.4
8.9 ± 0.4
3rd GradeNon-verbal
(TONI-4)
Word Recognition
Task (KTEA-II)
Decoding Task
(KTEA-II)
Reading
Comprehension
(KTEA-II)
NRNRNRNRNR
Lou C, et al. [29]2020CanadaEnglishRD random group6433:31:0010.9 ± 1.3NRNRSWE
(TOWRE)
PDE
(TOWRE)
Reading
Comprehension
(WJ III)
LettersNRNRNRNR
Liu T, et al. [30]2021FranceFrenchControl
DYX
31
26
13:18
13:13
11 ± 1
12 ± 1
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal (WISC)
Word Reading Fluency (Odedys);
Word Spelling-to-Dictation Test
Nonword Reading
Fluency (Odedys)
Alouette TestNRNRNRNRNR
Koirala N, et al. [31]2021USAEnglishRandom group244151:34:0010.2 ± 2.8NRFSIQ(WISC)SWE (TOWRE-2)PDE (TOWRE-2)NRNRElision and
Blending Words
(CTOPP-2)
NRNRNR
Huber E, et al. [32]2021USAEnglishControl
DYX
41
32
16:25
12:20
9.4
9.8
NRNRSWE (TOWRE)PDE (TOWRE)WJ-RFNRNRNRNRWJ-MFF; WJ-CALC; and WJ-BRS
Borghesani V, et al. [33]2021USAEnglishControl
DYX
14
26
5:9
14:12
10.4 ± 1.6
10.4 ± 2.0
1st grade
and 4th
grade
Non-verbal
(WASI)
SWE (TOWRE-2)PDE (TOWRE-2)Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT-5)NRNRNRNRNA
Vander Stappen C, et al. [34]2020FranceFrenchControl
DYX
13
18
5:8
9:9
10.5 ± 0.8
10.6 ± 1.0
NRNon-verbal
(WISC-IV)
SWE - BALEBALEBALEObjects; ColorsSyllable and
phoneme deletion task
NRNRNR
El-Sady S, et al. [35]2020EgyptArabicDYX2005:158.2 ± 1NRSB41 min reading DATnonsense passage
reading DAT
1 min reading DATObjectsPhonemic segmentation
subtest of DAT
NRNRBead threading;
Postural stability; and
DS
Wang HLS, et al. [36]2019TaiwanMandarin
Chinese
Control
DYX
22
24
NR9 ± 0.9
10 ± 1
Primary
school
Non-verbal
(WISC-IV)
Chinese
character
recognition
NRNRNRNRNRNRLexical tone
awareness;
auditory
identification
of FM test
Vanderauwera J, et al. [37]2019NetherlandsDutchTR and RD3419:1513.7 ± 0.5grade 8 (28),
7 (3)
and 9 (3)
WISC-III-NLOne-minute
word reading
test
Klepel testNRNANANANANR
Lou C, et al. [38]2019FranceFrenchControl
DYX
31
26
13:18
13:13
11.5 ± 1.4
11.6 ± 1.3
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal (WISC)
Word reading
test (Odedys)
Nonword
reading test
(Odedys)
Alouette testDigits;
Objects
Phoneme deletion
and spoonerism
Word spelling-
to-dictation test
NRVerbal WM
(DS- WISC)
Lebel C, et al. [39]2019USAEnglishDysfluent inaccurate Dysfluent accurate Non-impaired20
36
14
5:15
13:23
8:6
10.0 ± 1.2
9.4 ± 1.3
9.2 ± 1.2
NRWASI FSIQSWE (TOWRE);
LWID (WJ)
PDE (TOWRE);
WA (WJ)
Gray Oral
ReadingTest
(GORT-4)
NRPhonological
Decoding
(TOWRE)
NRNRNR
Banfi C, et al. [40]2018AustriaGermanTR
DYX
SD
27
21
21
12:15
9:12
6:15
9 ± 0.1
9 ± 0.3
10 ± 0.6
The end of
3rd and 4th
grade
Verbal and
Non-verbal
(WISC)
(SRLT-II);
Spelling (DRT-3)
(SRLT-II)Sentence
reading
fluency(SLS)
Digits;
Objects
PAVocabulary
standard score
(WISC-IV)
Parental
questionnaire ADHD
Verbal WM
and processing
speed (DS, Symbol
search WISC-IV)
Žarić G, et al. [41]2018NetherlandsDutchTR
DYX
13
15
8:5
7:8
9 ± 0.8
9 ± 0.6
2–3 years
of reading
instruction
Non-verbal
(WISC)
Word reading
subtest (3DM)
Pseudoword
reading subtest (3DM)
NRLetters;
Digits;
Objects
Phoneme deletion; Spelling;
and Letter speech
sound matching
NRNRMemory span
(syllables)
Su M, et al. [43]2018ChinaMandarin
Chinese
Control
DYX
22
18
11:11
7:11
11 ± 0.8
11 ± 1.0
Primary
school
Non-verbal and
Verbal (C-WISC)
Word list
reading; Chinese
character recognition
NRNRDigitsPhoneme deletionLexical decision;
Morphological
production
NRVerbal WM
(Digit recall)
Yagle K, et al. [42]2017USAEnglishTR
DYG
DYX
10
9
10
NR9–144–9
grades
Non-Verbal
(Wechsler)
Word reading
(TOSWRF); word
spelling (TOC)
Nonword readingNRNRNRNRNRNR
Christodoulou JA, et al. [44]2016USAEnglishTR
DYX
26
26
NR7.8 ± 0.6
7.8 ± 0.6
NRNon-verbal
(KBIT-2)
WID (WRMT-III);
SWE (TOWRE-2)
WA (WRMT-III);
PDE (TOWRE-2)
NRNRNRNRNRNR
Zhao JT, et al. [45]2016FranceFrenchControl
DYX
31
26
13:18
13:13
11.5 ± 1.3
11.6 ± 1.3
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal (WISC)
Word reading
fluency (Odedys);
Word spelling
Nonword reading
fluency (Odedys)
Alouette TestDigits, ObjectsWord spelling-
to-dictation test, Spoonerism
NRNRDS (WISC)
Koerte IK, et al. [46]2015GermanyGermanControl
DYX
24
16
0:24
0:16
9.9 ± 0.3
9.7 ± 0.4
3rd and
4th grades
Non-verbal
(CFT-20R)
SLRT-IISLRT-IINRDigits, Letters,
Colors, Objects
Phoneme
deletion
NRNRDS (K-ABC);
Verbal WM (Wechsler);
Arithmetic test
(HRT 1-4); and Number
line task
(WRT 1-4)
Garcia-Zapirain BG, et al. [47]2016SpainSpanishTR
DYX
MVR
19
20
18
8:11
8:12
8:10
10.0 ± 0.9
10.5 ± 1.1
10.4 ± 0.9
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal (WISC-IV)
Word reading (PROLEC-R)Pseudoword
reading (PROLEC-R)
ELFE 1-6NRNRNRNRWM
Fernandez VG, et al. [48]2016USAEnglishTR
DYX
27
29
15:12
14:15
10.1 ± 2.1
12.1 ± 2.5
6–8
grades
Verbal and
non-verbal (KBIT-2, SB4)
LWI (WJ-III);
WRAT-3; and
SWE (TOWRE)
PDE (TOWRE)PC (WJ-III-TA)NRNRNRNRNR
De Moura LM, et al. [49]2016BrazilPortugueseTR
RD
23
17
12:11
9:8
9.7 ± 0.9
9.2 ± 0.9
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal
(WISC-III)
Aloud reading
(TDE)
NRNRNRNRNRNRNR
Richards TL, et al. [50]2015USAEnglishControl
DYX
DYG
9
17
14
5:4
7:10
3:11
mean of 12.25
(from 9 to 15.6)
4–9
grades
Verbal
(WISC-IV)
Spelling dictated
words (WIAT III);
Sight Spelling (TOC)
NRNRNRNRNRNRBest and Fast
writing (DASH)
Marino C, et al. [51]2014ItalyItalianTR FRD+
TR FRD−
DYX FRD+
DYX FRD−
10
16
11
10
5:5
6:10
6:5
4:6
19.1 ± 1.9
18.7 ± 2.4
17.5 ± 2.4
16.4 ± 1.0
12.8 ± 1.5
12.0 ± 1.1
10.2 ± 1.8
10.8 ± 1.2
Full-scale
IQ (WISC-R)
Word reading(BVDDE);
Spelling (BVDDE)
Non-word
reading (BVDDE)
Sentences
containing
homophones
NRSpoonerism,
phonemic blending, and
syllable displacement (PA)
NRNRADC, letter and
number forward/backward
span (TEMA)
Fan Q, et al. [52]2014USAEnglishControl
DYX
20
19
9:11
8:11
12.0 ± 0.7
12.0 ± 0.7
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal
(WISC-IV)
LWID (WJ-III);
SWE (TOWRE); and
FLI and spelling
(WIST)
WA (WJ-III);
PDE (TOWRE)
PC and basic
reading (WJ-III);
TOSCRF
Color Digit
Objects (CTOPP)
NRNRNRNR
Fan Q, et al. [53]2014USAEnglishTR
RD
16
20
8:8
8:12
11.7 ± 0.7
12.1 ± 0.7
NRVerbal and
Non-verbal
(WISC-IV)
LWID (WJ-III);
SWE (TOWRE);
and FLI (WIST)
WA (WJ-III);
PDE (TOWRE)
NRNRWJ-III-PCNRNRNR
Hasan KM, et al. [54]2012USAEnglishTR
DYX
CFP
11
24
15
3:8
11:1
39:6
12.8 ± 1.7
13.7 ± 1.0
13.5 ± 0.8
NRComposite IQ
(KBIT-2, SB4)
LWID (WJ-III);
SWE (TOWRE)
PDE (TOWRE)PC (WJ-III)NRNRNRNRNR
Gebauer D, et al. [55]2012AustriaGermanControl
SI
SRI
11
11
9
NR12.3 ± 1.9
11.7 ± 1.6
11.3 ± 0.7
4th–5th
5–9 graders
Non-verbal
(Raven)
SLS 1-4 or 5-8;
Spelling (HSP)
SLS 1-4
or 5-8
ELFE 1-6NRNRNRNRPersonality assessment
FFQ (Asendropf)
Hoeft F, et al. [56]2011USAEnglishControl
DYX (rg)
DYX (nrg)
20
13
12
14:6
6:7
7:5
11.0 ± 2.6
14.5 ± 1.6
13.5 ± 2.2
NRNon-verbal
(WASI)
(WRMT) *;
SWE (TOWRE);
and Spelling and
writing fluency (WJ)
WA (WRMT);
PDE (TOWRE)
Gray Oral
ReadingTest(GORT);
PC (WRMT)
Colors; Objects;
Numbers; and Letters
NRPPVTNRMD (CTOPP)
Sihvonen AJ, et al. [57]2021FinlandFinnishControl
DYX
21
23
11:10
12:11
29.9 ± 6.0
31.3 ± 8.6
16.1 ± 4.4
15.7 ± 5.2
Verbal (WAIS-III);
PIQ (WAIS-IV)
Word List
Reading
Pseudoword
List Reading
Text ReadingTest not
specified
Pig Latin;
PA; phonological
short-term memory;
and rapid access
of information
NRASRS v1.1ARHQ;Verbal WM
(Non-word Span Length,
WMS-III)
Tschentscher N, et al. [58]2019GermanyGermanControl
DYX
12
12
0:12
0:12
23.7 ± 2.6
24.2 ± 2.3
NRNonverbal
(Raven)
SpellingNRReading speed
and
comprehension
Letters and
Numbers
NRNRNRNR
Moreau D, et al. [59]2018New ZealandEnglishControl
Dyscalc
DYX
Comorbid
11
11
11
12
4:7
5:6
5:6
5:7
27.7 ± 1.7
32 ± 2.2
29.4 ± 1.9
33.2 ± 1.7
15.2 ± 0.60
14.6 ± 0.56
15.6 ± 0.51
14.8 ± 0.61
FSIQ (WASI)WID (WJ)WA (WJ)NRNRNRWRAT spellingNRWRAT mathematics
Müller-Axt C, et al. [60]2017GermanyGermanControl
DYX
12
12
0:12
0:12
23.7 ± 2.6
24.2 ± 2.4
Undergraduate
students **
Non-verbal
(Raven)
SpellingNRNRNumbers;
Letters
NRNRNRNR
Vandermosten M, et al. [61]2013BelgiumDutchTR
DYX
20
20
12:8
13:7
21.4 ± 3.0
22.1 ± 3.1
NRNon-verbal
(WAIS-III)
Word reading;
Spelling
Pseudoword
reading
NRNRNRNRNRNR
Lebel C, et al. [62]2013USAEnglishRLD13664:13:0020.1 ± 3.1NRFSIQ (WASI)WID (WJ)WA (WJ)Fluency
(GORT)
NRNRNRNRNR
Vandermosten M, et al. [63]2012BelgiumDutchTR
DYX
20
20
12:8
13:7
21.4 ± 3.0
22.1 ± 3.1
NRNon-verbal
(WAIS)
Word reading;
Spelling
Pseudoword
reading
NRNRPA; Phoneme
deletion and
Spoonerism
Speech-in-
noise perception
(Dutch LIST)
NRNR
Frye RE, et al. [64]2011USAEnglishTR
DYX/PR
20
10
10:10
5:5
23.7 ± 0.7
23.9 ± 1.6
NRNon-verbal
(CTONI)
LWI (WJ-III);
Spelling
WA (WJ-III)Gray Oral
ReadingTest
(GORT)
Colors (CTOPP);
Digits (CTOPP);
Objects (CTOPP);
Letters (CTOPP)
PA (CTOPP);
APA (CTOPP)
NRTest of variables
of attention:
commissions,
omissions
NR
Note: Bold font indicates significant group differences. * time effect in DYX group, ** only 1 control had a high school diploma. Abbreviations: NA: Not Applicable; NR: Not Reported; FRD+: Children with Familial Risk for Dyslexia; FRD−: Children without Familial Risk for Dyslexia; TR: Typical reading; rg: reading gain; nrg: no rg; m: months; RAN: rapid automatized naming tasks; DYX: children with dyslexia; RD: Reading Disorder; RI: Reading Impairment; PreR: pre-reader children; BR: older reader children; FR: fluent reader children; PIQ: Performance IQ; FSIQ: Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; CFT 20-R: Cattell’s Fluid Intelligence Test, Scale 2; TrR: Treatment Responders; EF: Executive Functions; MD: Mood Disorders; WRD: word recognition deficits; RLD: reading and learning disabilities; SI: spelling impaired children; SRI: children with spelling and reading impairment; CFP: readers with comprehension or fluency problems; PA: Phonological awareness; PPVT: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; TONI or TONI-4: Test of Nonverbal Intelligence; CTONI: Comprehensive TONI; CTOPP: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; TOWRE or TOWRE-2: Test of Word Reading Efficiency; WJ: the Reading Fluency subtest of the Woodcock-Johnson Test; WISC or WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th edition; WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition; WIAT: Wechsler Individual Achievement Test; BRIEF: Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function; KBIT or KBIT-2: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; WRMT-R NU: Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised, Normative Update; WA: Word attention; SLRT-II: The Salzburg Reading and Spelling Test; YARC: York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension; GAPS: Grammar and Phonology Screening; CELF or CELF-4: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; KTEA-II: Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement-Second Edition; CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test; WID: word identification; LWID: letter and WID; VAS: Visual Attention Span; SB4: Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales-Fouth Edition; HLE: Home Literacy Environment; HOME: the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment; DAT: Dyslexia Assessment Test; ASRS: Adult Self Report Scale for ADHD clinical assessment; TOC: Test of Orthographic Competence; TOMAL2: Test of Memory and Learning - Second Edition; MSEL: Mullen Scales of Early Learning; ARHQ: Adult Reading History Questionnaire; ADC: Adult Dyslexia Checklist; HSP: Hamburger-Schreibprobe; SLS: Salzburger-Lese-Sreening; FFQ: five factor questionnaire; DAWBA: Diagnostic andWell-Being Assessment; SWE: SightWord Efficiency; DS: digit span; VSA: visual spatial attention; BISC: Bielefeld screening of literacy precursor abilities; DERET: German spelling test; SETK 3 5: a developmental German language test for children between 3 and 5 years of age; BAKO: Test of basic reading and spelling skills; PDE: Phonemic Decoding Efficiency; TOSWRF or TOSWRF-2: Test of Silent Word Reading Fluency, Second Edition; GORT: Grey Oral Reading Test; PC: Passage comprehension; ODEDYS: dyslexia screening tool; DKEFS: Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System; BALE: Analytic Battery of Written Language; DRT-3: Spelling percentile; 3DM: differential diagnostics for dyslexia; HRT: Heidelberger Rechentest; WRT: Weingartener Grundwortschatz Rechtschreibtest; PROLEC: Text Comprehension task; WRAT: Wide Range Achievement Test; TDE: test for School Achievement; DASH: Detailed Assessment of Speed of Handwriting; FLI: Fundamental Literacy Index; WIST: Word Identification and Spelling Test; ELFE: standardized achievement tests; TEMA: Test di Memoria e Apprendimento; BVDDE: Battery for the Assessment of Developmental Reading and Spelling Disabilities; and WM: working memory.
Table 2. DTI acquisition, image processing, and outcomes.
Table 2. DTI acquisition, image processing, and outcomes.
DTI AcquisitionDTI ProcessingDTI Outcomes
RefMRI FieldSequenceTR/TE
(ms)
Slice
Number
Slice Thickness
(mm)
FOV (mm)b-Value
(s/mm2)
N. of Diffusion
Gradients
TimeSoftwareCorrectionsType of
Analyses
DTI
Metrics
AtlasROIs/ TRACTSTracts Difference
between Groups
Clinical
Correlations
Zuk J, et al. [16]Siemens 3TDTINR302128x1280; 700NRNRDTIprep,
VISTALab
EC, HM (>2 mm or >0.5°)ROIFANRAF, SLF↑FA in r-SLF of
FHD+ TR compared to
FHD− TR and FHD+ RD
r-SLF FA, age, gender,
parent education, occupation,
and phonological awareness
significantly predicted decoding
skills among children FHD+
Yu X, et al. [17]Siemens 3TDTINRNR2NR0; 700; 1000NRNRDTIprep,
VISTALab,
AFQ
EC, HM (>2 mm/0.5°),
bed vibration,
pulsation, venetian
blind artifacts,
and slice and gradient-wise
intensity inconsistencies
Whole brain;
ROI
FAMNI,
Native space
Right of SLF,
ILF, and AF,
sCC, CC2
↑FA in r-sCC
of FHD+ TR compared
to FHD− TR
R-sCC FA had
positive correlation
with r-IFG
activation for
FHD−/+ TR
Langer N, et al. [18]Siemens 3TDTI8320/88642256x2561000305:59 minDTIprep,
FSL (DTIFIT),
Trackvis
(Diffusion Toolkit),
Trackvis,
AFQ
EC and
HM (>2 mm and 0.5°)
Whole brain;
ROI
FA
RD
AD
MNIBilateral AF
and CS
↓FA in l-AF (central
portion) FHD+ compared
with FHD−, corrected
by age
l-AF FA has positive
correlation with age,
expressive language
Kraft I, et al. [19]Siemens 3TDTI8000/NR661.9NR10006032 minFSL (Topup tool),
FSL (DTIFIT),
MRTrix
EC, HM, and
susceptibility-
induced distortions
ROIFADestrieux AtlasSMG, ITG
(anterior, long,
and posterior AF),
SOS/TOS, IFoG,
IFobG
No group differencel-aAF was the
best predictor
of DYX
Vanderm- osten M, et al. [20]Philips 3TDTI7600/65582.5200x24013006010 min 32 sExplore DTI,
Trackvis
EC, HM (6 parameters)
Reorientation of the b-matrix
Motion as covariate
Whole brain;
ROI
FATrackVisAF (dorsal FTP,
dorsal post TP),
ventral IFOF
↓FA in l-IFOF
of FHD+
Phonological awareness
positive correlation
with FA of l-AF(TP)
and bilateral IFOF/AF-FTP,
as also left ventral
tracts in FHD+
Van Der Auwera S, et al. [21]Philips 3TDTI7600/65NR2.5NR13006010:32 minFSL,
VISTALab,
AFQ
EC, HM by root
mean square
Whole brain;
ROI
FA
MD
NRAF↓FA in the
l-AF in pre-reader
RDs
aAF FA was a
significant predictor
for scores on word
reading tests from
2nd grade
Wang Y, et al. [22]Siemens 3TDTI8320/88NRNR256x2561000305:59 minDTIprep,
VISTALab,
AFQ
EC,
HM (>2 mm and >0.5°)
Whole brain;
ROI
FA
AD
RD
white matter
atlas
Left of AF,
SLF, ILF
↓l-AF FA at
pre-reader FHD+ versus
FHD− and for poor
versus good readers
all ages; FHD+ good
readers had faster
WM development in
r-SLF compared to
poor readers
l-AF and ILF FA
positive correlations
with word identification
skill
Vanderau- wera J, et al. [23]Philips 3TDTI7600/65NR2.5NR13006010:32 minExploreDTI,
Trackvis
EC, HMROIFAnative spaceLong, anterior and
posterior dorsal AF,
and ventral IFOF
↑FA in all groups
over time. ↓ long
AF FA in DYX prior to
reading onset, right
also kept in early
reading. Influence of
FHD+ in l-IFOF and
long r-AF
FHD+ and rapid naming
predicted 80.3% of cases;
the l-longAF FA values
predicted 84.4% of
DYX cases
Zhao J. et al. [24]Siemens 3TDTI14,000/91701.721814006018 minExplore DTI,
Trackvis,
FSL
NRWhole brainFATrackVis
MNI-152
UF, FATMales DYX had a ↓HMOA
in the UF compared
with males TR
HMOA of the UF
showed a positive correlation
with VAS in DYXs
Meisler SL, [25]Siemens 3TDKI3320/100.2NR1.8NR0; 1000; 200064NRQSIPrep,
MRtrix, FSL, and
TractSeg
Gibbs unringing,
EC, HM,
and AP-PA field
Whole brainFAFSL and
MNI
AF, SLF (I, II, and III),
ILF, IFOF, UF, SCP,
ICP, MCP, and sCC
No group differenceAge and sex with gFA positive
correlation; in older children,
FA in r-SLF and l-ICP
related to nonword
reading skills
Liu T, et al. [26]Siemens 3TDTI14,000/91701.721814006018 minPANDA,
FSL, and
Trackvis
ECWhole brainFAMNI and
AAL atlas
90 ROIs of AALNRPositive correlation
between node FA for
l-SOG and VAS score, l-MTG
and l-ORBsupmed and
phonological score
Farah R, et al. [27]Philips 3TDTI6652.446/82.61602224x120x2241000617 min 25 sVISTALab, AFQEC, HMWhole brain;
ROI
FANRAF, SLF, ILF↓ FA in the left
of AF, ILF, and SLF
in RD
↓ FA in the l-SLF
positive correlated with
reading and working memory
score in DYX
Partanen M, et al. [28]GE 3TDTI7000/60602256x2560; 1000607.5 minTORTOISE,
FDT (FSL),
DTIFIT (FSL), and
PROBTRACKX (FSL)
EC, HMWhole brain;
ROI
FA
MD
MNI305 and
Desikan–Killiany
atlas
bilateral IFG, Ins,
STG, SMG, AnG, and FFG
↑MD in bilateral Ins;
l-IFtG, l-STG, and
r-SMG in DYX
SMG, r-IFoG, and l-Ins MD
had negative correlation
with reading gains and
decoding, respectively
Lou C, et al. [29]Siemens 3TDTI3000/50.6642256x2560; 100056NRExploreDTIEC, HM,
EPI distortions
Whole brain;
ROI
FAAAL and
MNI152
90 ROIs of AALNRIFtG and IFoG, Ins,
FFG, IPL, SMG, AnG,
HG, STG, MTG, ITG,
IOG, PreCG, ROL, and
thalamus in the left
hemisphere positive correlated
with reading efficiency and
phonemic decoding, mainly
for girls DYX
Liu T, et al. [30]Siemens 3TDTI14,000/91701.7218x2180; 14006018 minPANDA,
FSL
EC, HMWhole brain;
ROI
FAAAL and MNI90 ROIs of AALNRNegative correlation between
READACC (pseudoword/word
reading) and the r-FFG
FA in DYX
Koirala N, et al. [31]Siemens 3TDTINRNR1.8NR0; 1000; 200064NRFSL (QUAD),
FSL (DTIFIT), and
FSL (BEDPOSTX),
XTRACT
Susceptibility,
EC, and HM
Whole brain;
ROI
FA
MD
RD
ODI
NDI
Native space23 tracts (including SLF,
which seeds were
central sulcus, SFG,
ACG, MFG, and AnG)
NRPositive correlation
between phonological processing
and the left IFOF, MDLF,
SLF2, VOF, CBD and FX FA,
and the l- UF MD
Huber E, et al. [32]Phillips 3TDKINRNR2NR0; 800; 200032 and 64NRFSL, DIPY,
MRTrix, and AFQ
AP-PA, EC,
Mean slice-by-slice
displacement > 3 mm
Whole brainFA
MD
AWF
Da
MDe
NRAF, CS, UF, SLF, ILF,
ThR, FMj, FMn, and IFOF
l-AF MD difference for
Group x time interaction
Positive correlation between
MD of l-AF, UF, l-ILF, l-IFOF,
FMj, MDe of left of AF, UF,
ILF, IFOF, and FMj with word
reading and negative correlation
between AWF of right ILF, IFOF, and
FMn with word reading
Borghesani V, et al. [33]Siemens 3TDKI8200/86602.2220x2200; 700; 20,00030 and 6415 minFSL (NODDI model),
FS-TRACULA
AP-PA, EC, and HMVoxel-based;
ROI
NDI
ODI
FSL,
Desikan–Killiany
Atlas and MNI
l-VOT↑ODI in DYS at
the l-VOT
NR
Vander Stappen C, et al. [34]Philips 3TDTI6422/83702224x22480055NRBrainVoyagerEC, HMROIFATalairach spaceAF, IFOF, and ILFNRRAN Gains negative correlated
with FA in the l-long aAF,
and the r-pAF, a reduction
in naming times was linked
to an increase in FA in those
tracts at DYX
El-Sady S, et al. [35]Philips 1.5TDTINR702230x230NR32NRNREC, HMROIFA
ADC
NRSLF, AF, CR, PLIC of CSNRNegative correlation between
r-AF FA and at-risk quotient,
l-sCR ADC with writing, and r-SLF
ADC with bDS and positive with
VF.Positive correlation between
l-SLF-aCR FA and RAN, spelling,
and VF, as r-PLIC ADC with writhing,
and l-aCR ADC with bDS
Wang HLS, et al. [36]Siemens 3TDTI6700/97NR2.7NR5000128NRDSI StudioNRWhole brainNRMNI,
AAL atlas
IFOF, CC, cerebellar, and
Tha-pontine tracts
NRl-IFOF, cerebellar, and Tha-pontine
tracts had positive correlated with
chinese character recognition;
pCC association with auditory
FM processing in DD
Vanderauwera J, et al, [37]Phillips 3TDTI8872/2.5552.5240x240x137.510006013:52 minExploreDTI,
Trackvis
HM (>1.5 mm) and ECROIFANative SpaceAF, IFOF, UF, and ILFNRWord reading had positive
correlation with l-long-AF
FA and negative with l-long-AF RD
and UF RD. Paternal educational level
had positive correlation with
l-long AF FA, and UF FA; after covariate by HM,
only the l-UF remained significant
Lou C, et al. [38]Siemens 3TDTI14,000/91701.72180; 14006018 min
(3x6 min)
ExploreDTI,
FSL (FLIRT)
EC, HMWhole brainFAAAL atlas;
MNI;
Harvard-Oxford atlas
Left of MTG-MOG,
MOG-TPOsup, TPOsup-HG,
HG-ROL, Ins-ROL,
STG-Ins, and Ins-SMG
↓mean FA in DYX for
all ROIs
Literacy skills had
positive correlation with
clustering coefficient, local
efficiency, transitivity, and
global efficiency, in DYX
Lebel C, et al. [39]Siemens 1.5TSE EPI9000/85285240x2401000NR7:24 minFSLMotion artifacts
(signal drop out,
venetian blind artifact,
and mechanical vibration
artifact, >10), EC
ROIFA
MD
AD
RD
MNI;
JHU ICBM-
DTI-81 atlas
sCC, ALIC of CS,
aCR, pCR, SS (includes
the ILF and IFOF),
UF, and SLF
↓MD in r-CR, and
l-UF in DYX
Age had a positive correlation
with pCR, r-SLF FA, negative
with pCR, l-UF MD. Sight
words and VF were positively
correlated with l-SLF FA and
MD, respectively, as well as with l-pCR
MD. Phonological decoding had a
negative correlation with r-pCR MD
and mean MD and positive
with mean FA
Banfi C, et al. [40]Siemens 3TDTI3400/105482.52400; 200064NRMRTrix,
FSL, and
AFQ
AP-PA, EC,
HM, and susceptibility-
induced distortion
Whole brainFANRThR, FMj, FMn, IFOF,
ILF, SLF, and AF,
UF, CS, and CG
↑FA in ILF, r-SLF,
and r-CG in DYX
Negative correlation between
r-ILF FA and reading measures,
controlling for spelling.
Žarić G, et al. [41]Siemens 3TDTI10,800/84(protocol1)
11,000/85(protocol2)
851.8NR0; 10007215 minVISTALab
(mrDiffusion),
SPM, AFQ
EC, HM and
phase-encoding
direction corrections
Whole brain;
ROI
FANRAF, SLF, ILF,
IFOF, UF, lCS,
antThR, FMn, and FMj
↑FA in the AF,
r-SLF, and aThR
in DYX
r-SLF showed age effects
that differed between groups.
Age effect in ILF FA,
and CC (FMj and FMn). L-aThR
positive correlation with
age appropriate reading
accuracy scores
Su M, Zhao J, et al. [43]Siemens 3TDTI8000/89NR2.2282x2820; 100030NR
(repeated twice)
ExploreDTI,
Trackvis, and
FSL
EC and HMWhole brain;
ROI
FA
RD
AD
MNI152;
native space
AF, IFOF, and ILF↓FA and AD in
the l-AF and
I-ILF in DYX
AF and ILF FA positive
correlation with character
recognition, digit recall,
phoneme deletion (only AF),
and morphological production
(only ILF). ILF FA negative
correlation with RAN
Yagle K, et al. [42]NRDTI8593/78NR2220x220x1280; 1000329:35 minFSLNRROIFA
RA
AD
RD
MD
NROR, CS, ILF,
SLF, and CG
↓FA in l-OR
in DYX
NR
Christod- oulou JA, et al. [44]Siemens 3TDTI9300/847422560; 70030NRFS-TRACULA,
DTIprep, and
FSL (FLIRT)
EC, HMTract-basedFA
AD
RD
MNI152SLF, AF↓FA in the
l-AF in RDs
Positive correlation of
l-AF FA and negative DA
with real-word reading
Zhao JT, et al. [45]Siemens 3TDTI14,000/91701.72180; 14006018 minExploreDTI,
FSL, and
Trackvis
Motion correctionsWhole brain;
ROI
HMOA
FA
MNI152IFOF, ILF, SLF, and AF↓FA of r-IFOF
and l-SLF in DYX
r-IFOF FA negative
correlation with reading and
spelling accuracy
Koerte IK, et al. [46]Siemens 3TNR9600/1106522080; 100030NR3DSlicer,
FSL (FLIRT), and
FSL (TBSS)
EC, HMTract-basedFA
AD
RD
trace
MNI152NRNo group differencePositive correlation
arithmetic test with FA
and AD and negative with
RD (Temporo-parietal)
Garcia-Zapirain BG, et al. [47]Philips 3TDTI6819/81602224x22480015 7minFSL (BET),
FSL (FDT), and
FSL (TBSS)
NRWhole-brain;
ROI
FA
MD
AD
RD
MNI; Atlas JHU
White- matter
CC, SLF, ILF,
lower FOF, l-AF,
IFOF
↓FA in l-AF in DYXNR
Fernandez VG, et al. [48]Philips 3TDTI6100/84443240x2400; 100021NRFSL (DTIFIT)EC, HMROIFA
AD
RD
Desikan and
Destrieux atlases
LAC/RAC to bilateral
TP, OT, and IFG
↑FA of cerebellar to
TP and IFG; ↓RD in TP
in DYX
FA of AC-OT had interaction
between age and group,
younger DYX have ↓FA in
this region.
De Moura LM, et al. [49]GE 1.5TDTI11,600/99473240x2400; 80015NRFSL,
FSL(TBSS)
EC correction
and non brain
voxels removed
Voxel-basedFA
RD
MD
AD
MNI152aThR, CG, CS,
IFOF, ILF, UF,
FMj, FMn, and CGH
↓FA left of aThR, CG,
CS, FMj, FMn, UF, right of
IFOF, ILF ↑RD in the
left of CG, CS, and SLF
in DYX
NR
Richards TL, et al. [50]Philips 3TDTI8593/78NR2.0220x220x1280; 1000329 min 35 sDTIPre (GTRAC),
FSL (TBSS), and
FSL
NRROIFA
AD
RD
RA
MD
FSL white matter
atlas (FHU)
aThR, FMn, CS,
SLF, ILF, IFOF,
UF, and CG
↓RA in aThR, IFOF, SLF, UF,
and l-CG, and FMn; ↓AD in
CS, r-ThR, CG, IFOF, SLF, and UF
in DYX
NR
Marino C, et al. [51]Philips 3TDTI9775/58NR2.3NR0; 100035NRBrainVoyager
(Brainvisa),
SPM
EC, smooth 6 mmVoxel-basedFAWhite matter
atlases of FSL
ILF, IFOF, AF,
SLF, CC, and OR
NRDYX with DCDC2d gene
x without found ↓FA in
ILF and l-CC
Fan Q, et al. [52]Philips 3TDTI6237/75602.2212x2120; 700323 min 32 sFSL (FDT),
FS-TRACULA
EC, HMROIFADesikan–Killiany
Atlas
Thalamus to OFC,
MPFC, LPFC, SMC,
PC, MTC, LTC, OCC,
and Ins
↑FA of LPFC and
SMC to ThR in DYX
Th-SMC showed negative
correlation with basic
reading score
Fan Q, et al. [53]Philips 3TDTI6237/75602.2212x212700323 min 38sFSLEC, HMROINRMNI1525 ROIs of l-OT/F,
MTG, ITG, LOCC,
PaHipp, and ILF
Left Mid, Inf and
sup- TG, lingual, fusiform,
Sup and Inf PG in DYX
NR
Hasan KM, et al. [54]Philips 3TDTI6100/84443NR100021minNRNRROIFA
MD
AD
RD
Dav
NRCC↑mFA of CC
in DYX
MD and AD correlation
with age (CC2); MD positive
correlated with Letter-
Word ID test in CC5
Gebauer D, et al. [55]Siemens 3TDTI6700/95352.5250NRNRNRFSL (TBSS, FDT,
DTIFIT, and BET)
ECVoxel-basedFAJHU ICBM-DTI-81
White-Matter Labels
aCR, CC↓FA in the
l-aCR and aCC
NR
Hoeft F, et al. [56]GE 3TDTI11,600/64.523424080013NRSPM,
DTIStudio, and
ROQS
EC, HMWhole-brainFANRSLFNRPositive correlation
between r-SLF FA
and single-word reading
Sihvonen AJ, et al. [57]Siemens 3TDTI9000/80702.5240x2400; 100064NRMRTrix,
DSI Studio
Thermal noise
with MP-PCA,
Gibbs ringing
correction
Whole brainQAMNI using
(QSDR)
NR↓QA in VOF, SLF, AF,
CC, CSl-UF, and ThR;
↑QA in l-SLF, VOF, and
CS in DYX
Reading skill positive
association with l-CG and
right fornix, and frontal
corticopontine tracts
and cerebellum
Tschentscher N, et al. [58]Siemens 3TDTI12,900/100881.7220x2200; 1000606 minFSL (FDT),
FSL (PROBTRACKX),
and FSL (BEDPOSTX)
Head motion
corrections
Voxel-based;
ROI
FAMNI;
Juelich histological;
Harvard-Oxford atlases
A1, l-mPT, and MGB, IC↓connectivity between
l-mPT-MGB in DYX
Negative correlation of
l- mPT-MGB with reading
skills in TR
Moreau D, et al. [59]Siemens 1.5TDTI6601/101NR32300; 100030NRFSL (DTIFIT),
FSL (FLIRT),
and FSL (TBSS)
EC and
motion corrections
Whole brain;
Voxel-based
FAMNI152Bilateral CR and AFNo group differenceNR
Müller-Axt C, et al. [60]Siemens 3TDTI12,900/100881.7220x2200; 1000606 minFSLMotion correctionROIFATalairach;
MNI;
Juelich
Histological atlas
LGN, l-V1, V5/MT↓LGN FA and between
l-V5/MT-LGA in DYX
DYX showed negative correlation
between l- V5/MT-LGN and
name letters and numbers
aloud time
Vanderm- osten M, et al. [61]Philips 3TDTI11,043/55682.2220x2200; 8004521 min 8 sExplore DTI,
FSL (CATNAP)
EC and motion-
induced artifacts
Whole-brain;
ROI
FAHarvard-Oxford atlas
in MNI space
Post STG, AF, sCCNRPositive correlation
between coherence 20 Hz
and FA of the STGp Lat and
sCC in DYX and a negative
in HC, without outliers
Lebel C, et al. [62]Siemens 1.5TDTI9000/85285240x2400; 100067 min 24 sSPMSmooth of
4 mm kernel
Voxel-basedFA
MD
ICBM templateALIC, sCC, ThR,
CR, ILF, IFOF, anf aCR
NRGORT fluency positive
correleted with FA of aCC,
sCC, right: aLimb, SLF, MCP,
aCR, ILF, l-sCC, Th, IFOF;
Word attack with FA of aCC,
SLF, aLimb; l-Th, SLF, and r-IFOF
Vandermosten M, et al. [63]Philips 3TDTI11,043/55682.2220x2200; 8004521 min 8 sExplore DTI,
FSL (CATNAP)
EC, motion-induced
artifacts correction
ROIFA
RD
AD
Native spaceAF, IFOF↓FA of l-AF
in DYX
Direct and l-aAF FA positive
correlated with phoneme
awareness, and speech
perception, respectively, and
l-IFOF with orthography
Frye RE, et al. [64]Philips 3TDTI6100/84443240x2401000NR7 minSPMDistortion correction,
masking, and isotropic
voxel interpolation
Whole-brainFA
AD
RD
Dav
ICBMFTP, SLF, SFOF, IFOF, and CRNo group differenceNegative correlated: FA-
word attack in SLF, SFOF, aCR,
and pCR; Dav- word attack
in SLF; and positive correlation:
Dav and AD—word attack
in SFOF
Abbreviations: Ref.: Reference; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; N: number; DTI: diffusion tensor image; DKI: diffusion kurtosis imaging; NR: Not reported; TR: Time of repetition; TE: time of echo; FOV: field of view; b: diffusion weighting; l: left; r: right; UF: Uncinate fasciculus; FAT: frontal aslant tract; EC: Eddy Current; ICBM: International Consortium for Brain Mapping; SLF: Superior longitudinal fasciculus; SFOF: superior frontal–occipital fasciculus.; IFOF: inferior frontal–occipital fasciculus; ROQS: Reproducible Objective Quantification Scheme; CC: corpus callosum; CC5: posterior midbody of CC; CC2: genu of CC; sCC: splenium of CC; aCC: anterior CC; VOT: ventral occipitotemporal cortex; OT/F: occipitotemporal/fusiform; SMC: supramarginal cortex; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; FTP: frontal-temporo-parietal regions; TP: temporo-parietal regions; FMj: forceps major; FMn: forceps minor; ThR: thalamic radiations; aThR: anterior ThR; CG: cingulum; CS: corticalspinal tract; AF: arcuate fasciculus; aAF: anterior AF; pAF: posterior AF; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; HMOA: Hindrance-modulated oriented anisotropy; VAS: Visual Attention Span; AFQ: Automated Fiber Quantification software; SCP: superior cerebellar peduncle; ICP: inferior cerebellar peduncle; MCP: middle cerebellar peduncle; FA: fractional anisotropy, gFA: global white matter fractional anisotropy; AAL: automated anatomical labeling; MNI: Montreal Neurological Institute; SOG: superior occipital gyrus; MTG: middle temporal gyrus; ORBsupmed: medial orbital superior frontal gyrus; TR: typical readers; RD: reading disorder; QSDR: q-space diffeomorphic reconstruction; QA: quantitative anisotropy; FDT: FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox; MD: mean diffusivity; STG: superior temporal gyrus; ITG: inferior temporal gyrus; SMG: supramarginal gyrus; DYX: dyslexia; AnG: angular gyrus; IPL: inferior parietal lobe; IOG: inferior occipital gyrus; PreCG: precentral gyrus; ROL: Rolandic operculum; FFG: fusiform gyrus; QUAD: Quality Assessment of dMRI; DTIfit: diffusion tensor modeling tool; MDLF: middle longitudinal fasciculus; VOF: ventral occipital fasciculus; CBD: dorsal cingulum; FX: fornix; AWF: axonal water fraction; Da: intra-axonal diffusivity; MDe: extra-axonal mean diffusivity; NDI: neurite density index; ODI: orientation dispersion index; FHD+: positive familial risk to develop dyslexia; FHD−: negative familial risk to develop dyslexia; CR: corona radiata; aCR: anterior CR; pCR: posterior CR; PLIC: posterior limb of internal capsule; FM: frequency modulation; A1: primary auditory cortex; mPT: planum temporale; MGB: medial geniculate body; IC: inferior colliculus; BEDPOSTX: Bayesian Estimation of Diffusion Parameters Obtained using Sampling Techniques; MOG: middle occipital gyrus; -TPOsup: temporal pole; HG: Heschl’s gyrus; AD: axial diffusivity; RD: radial diffusivity; ALIC: anterior limb of the internal capsule; SD: spelling disorder; TBSS: Tract-based spatial statistics; FLIRT: FMRIB linear image registration tool; RA: relative anisotropy; SLD: specific learning disability; WM: white matter; TP: temporoparietal; LGN: lateral geniculate nucleus; V1: primary visual cortex; V5/MT: middle temporal area; TRACULA: TRActs Constrained by UnderLying Anatomy; TP-AF: temporo-paietal portion of the AF; ASD: autism spectrum disorder; SOS/TOS: superior and transversal occipital sulci; BET: Brain Extraction Tool; LAC: left anterior cerebellum; RAC: right anterior cerebellum; HC: health control; JHU: Johns Hopkins School; OFC: orbitofrontal cortex; MPFC: medial prefrontal cortex; LPFC: lateral prefrontal cortex; PC: parietal cortex; MTC: medial temporal cortex; LTC: lateral temporal cortex; OCC: occipital cortex; Ins: insular cortex; LOCC: lateral OCC; PaHipp: parahippocampal regions; GORT: Gray Oral Reading Test; aLimb: anterior limb; SS: sagital stratum; IC: inferior colliculus; OR: Opptic radiation; IFoG: pars opercularis of inferior frontal gyrus; IFtG: pars triangularis of IFG; IFobG: pars orbitalis of IFG; bDS: backward digit span; and VF: verbal fluency.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Martins, B.; Baba, M.Y.; Dimateo, E.M.; Costa, L.F.; Camara, A.S.; Lukasova, K.; Nucci, M.P. Investigating Dyslexia through Diffusion Tensor Imaging across Ages: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 349. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14040349

AMA Style

Martins B, Baba MY, Dimateo EM, Costa LF, Camara AS, Lukasova K, Nucci MP. Investigating Dyslexia through Diffusion Tensor Imaging across Ages: A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(4):349. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14040349

Chicago/Turabian Style

Martins, Bruce, Mariana Yumi Baba, Elisa Monteiro Dimateo, Leticia Fruchi Costa, Aila Silveira Camara, Katerina Lukasova, and Mariana Penteado Nucci. 2024. "Investigating Dyslexia through Diffusion Tensor Imaging across Ages: A Systematic Review" Brain Sciences 14, no. 4: 349. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14040349

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop